• Non ci sono risultati.

Guarda Il lato positivo. Una ricerca sul mind-wandering nelle situazioni di insegnamento-apprendimento

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Condividi "Guarda Il lato positivo. Una ricerca sul mind-wandering nelle situazioni di insegnamento-apprendimento"

Copied!
22
0
0

Testo completo

(1)

Fo rm az io ne & I ns eg na m en to X V II I 3 20 20 C od ic e IS SN 2 27 9-75 05 ( on li ne ) © P en sa M ul ti M ed ia E di to re C od ic e do i: 10 .7 34 6/ -f ei -X V II I-03 -2 0_ 20

The positive side: an inquiry into mind-wanderingùin

tea-ching-learning situations

Il lato positivo. Una ricerca sul mind-wandering

nelle situazioni di insegnamento-apprendimento

ABSTRACT

The article addresses an overlooked topic in the field of educational sci-ences: teachers’ experience about mind-wandering, and presents a study that analyzes the dynamics of the psychic phenomenon by a combination of a new methodology and Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis (IPA). First, we present the phenomenon of mind-wandering as a relevant object of study, then we suggest that it can play a positive role in teaching-learning activities.

We aim to provide a richer picture of the process that is commonly under-stood as a shift in attention. Through the discussion based on real-life field study on seven primary and secondary Italian school teachers, we show how mind-wandering is a relevant psychological phenomenon, a space of explo-ration and reflection where teachers rethink completely their role with re-gard to the students, and reconsider their mental process engaged in planning their own work. Thanks to the proposed methodology, one can gain a deeper access to teachers’ subjectivity, both as individuals absorbed in learning situations but also in their experiences, beliefs, images and social representations of mind-wandering that are connected to a specific cultural context, fieldwork, and activities of meaning construction.

L'articolo affronta un argomento trascurato nel campo delle scienze della formazione, l'esperienza degli insegnanti del mind-wandering. Si propone lo studio delle dinamiche del fenomeno psichico con una nuova metodolo-gia combinata all’Interpretative Phenomenical Analysis. Inizialmente, pre-sentiamo l’oggetto di studio, il mind- wandering, e successivamente, vediamo come possa svolgere un ruolo positivo nelle attività di insegna-mento-apprendimento.

Nadia Dario

IBEF - Ideas for the Basic Education of the Future, Shangai - info@nadiadario.it

Luca Tateo

IBEF - Ideas for the Basic Education of the Future, Shangai - lucatateo@gmail.com

* Il manoscritto è il risultato di un lavoro collaborativo degli autori, il cui specifico contributo è da

ri-ferirsi come segue: i paragrafi 1,2,3,4 sono da attribuirsi a Nadia Dario; introduzione e conclusioni a Luca Tateo; The manuscript is the result of a joint effort on the part the authors whose specific contribution is indicated as follows: paragraphs 1,2,3,4 were contributed by Nadia Dario; introduc-tion and conclusions by Luca Tateo.

(2)

Introduction

In everyday life, conscious experience is changeable: it rarely remains on one task

or on one topic for an extended period without deviations. In some cases, the

shift in focus can be described in terms of mind-wandering. According to the most

common definitions in experimental psychology, mind-wandering (MW) is an

at-tention shift from a current task to ‘‘task-unrelated thought”, ‘‘task-unrelated

im-ages and thoughts”, ‘‘stimulus-independent thought”, ‘‘mind pops”, or ‘‘zone outs”

(Philips, Mills, D’ Mello & Risko, 2016; Jha, Morrison, Dainer-Best, Parker, Rostrup,

Staley, 2015; Smallwood, 2013; Smallwood & Schooler, 2006; Singer, 1966;1974;

Antrobus et al. 1966). MW has a number of definitions and dimensions, which

seem to encompass a broad range of phenomena, and to predict a number of

im-portant functions and outcomes: social cognition, mental imagery, inner speech,

guilt, fear of failure, poor attentional control (McMillan, Kaufman, & Singer, 2013;

Meta Regis, 2013; Seli, Beaty, Marty-Dugas, & Smilek, 2019; Fossa, Gonzalez, & Di

Montezemolo, 2019).

In literature, one can encounter two main perspectives on MW: on the one

hand, mind-wandering has been associated with beneficial processes such as

goal-directed thinking, planning and creativity. On the other hand, it correlates

with costly outcomes such as attenuated processing of the environment, driving

accidents, learning disruption, affective dysfunction and impaired performance

in daily life (Dario & Tateo, 2019).

When it comes to formal education and to teaching-learning practices, many

of them are still largely based on the assumption that continuous attention is

fun-damental in the classroom, and that one of the teacher’s tasks is to control and

maintain students’ focused attention, avoiding “distractions”. However, the

ubiq-uitous nature of a psychic phenomenon such as mind-wandering, led the authors

of this study to wonder if it is not simply a matter of “distraction”, and if this

phe-nomenon plays a more central role in teaching-learning. We think that further

theoretical and empirical explorations are thus required, not only to understand

the complex phenomenon of MW in relation to the other psychic experiences,

Il nostro obiettivo è fornire un quadro approfondito di questo processo che viene comunemente inteso come uno spostamento dell'attenzione. Attra-verso la discussione dello studio condotto nel contesto di vita reale di sette insegnanti italiani della scuola primaria e secondaria, mostriamo come il mind-wandering sia un fenomeno psicologico di rilievo, uno spazio di esplo-razione e riflessione dove gli insegnanti ripensano il proprio ruolo nei con-fronti degli studenti, un processo mentale impegnato nella pianificazione del loro lavoro. Grazie alla metodologia proposta si ha un accesso immediato alla soggettività degli insegnanti in quanto individui assorbiti sia dalle situa-zioni di apprendimento sia dalle loro esperienze, credenze, immagini e rap-presentazioni sociali del mind-wandering che sono connesse ad uno specifico contesto culturale e lavorativo e all’attività di costruzione di signi-ficato.

KEYWORDS

mind-wandering; cultural psychology of education; new methodology, IPA analysis

mind-wandering; psicologia culturale dell’educazione; nuova metodologia, analisi IPA

(3)

but also to evaluate its positive and negative aspects in everyday experience. This

study is part of a more detailed research project studied in different educational

teaching-learning situations: lecturing, cooperative learning and testing. In the

ar-ticle we are interested in addressing teachers’ experience about mind-wandering,

and we present a study that analyzes the dynamics of the psychic phenomenon

through a combination of a new observation methodology (Dario & Tateo, 2019)

and Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis (IPA). In the first part of the article,

we treat the phenomenon of mind-wandering as a relevant object of study in

ed-ucational sciences, then we suggest that it can play a positive role in

teaching-learning activities. Through the discussion based on a real-life field study on seven

primary and secondary Italian school teachers, we show how mind-wandering is

a relevant psychological phenomenon, a space of exploration and reflection

where teachers rethink completely their role with regard to students, and

recon-sider their mental process engaged in planning their own work.

1. Mind-wandering in learning-teaching settings: new research questions

There is not much attention in educational sciences paid to mind-wandering

1

.

This lack of interest toward this topic seems unjustified, in particular if one

con-siders the impact of the phenomenon in psychic life: MW occurs in approximately

half of thoughts-content when awake (Killingsworth and Gilbert, 2010). In

educa-tion, where mind-wandering is common (e.g. any teacher has probably observed

students while they are partaking in an “absent minded job, as if they have

stopped in time”: account of primary school pupils during a focus group), the

stud-ies are limited to literacy skills (reading and comprehension); memory (Martin,

Mills, D’Mello, Risko, 2019; Philips et al. 2016); to a single primary task and at

uni-versity settings (Wammes et al. 2019); to academic performance (Lindquist &

McLean, 2011); or to manipulation of a single factor as the “live teacher presence”

(Varao-Sousa and Kingstone, 2015). As claimed by Irving and Thompson (2018),

the mainstream cognitive research, in its definition of the phenomenon, does not

account for the dynamics of MW (i.e. the task, the unrelated thoughts that are not

mind-wanderings, and the ways in which MW can be task-related). We suggest

that this limited interest and few real-life empirical studies in school context are

due to a taken for granted: mind-wandering equals distraction.

In this article, we claim instead that MW is a relevant psychic process. To

ap-preciate its real functioning in educational activities, it must be studied in its

micro-genetic development, during the activity in real-life learning and teaching

settings. In particular, the direct experimental methods such as probe-caught and

self-caught, so far used in ecological studies, tend to stop the ongoing activity,

and there are no elements able to re-conduct the subject to the actual MW

expe-rience. Hence, we felt the need to elaborate a new methodology for studying this

1 A search on Scopus and WOS Databases, using the terms “mind-wandering”, “daydreaming”, in the fields “Title, Abstract, Keywords” shows that much of the research on the subject developed after the publication of the seminal work of Smallwood et al. (2006), and it is linked to the control struc-tures of the states of consciousness of the subject (control processes) and cognitive process. This is confirmed by the greatest number of publication emerging among others on “Consciousness and Cognition”. The contributions of learning and teaching are connected with the theories of learning where we can find an interest in cognitive studies and few studies on mw connected with theories of education or training. Szpunar, Moulton and Schacter (2013) provide a non-systematic review of the literature in the field, yet still with the idea of curbing the occurrence of MW in education.

(4)

phenomenon in the environments where learning takes place, and one can

ob-serve more than one factor in action: teacher presence, materiality, social setting,

multiple perspectives. In this perspective, a number of new potential research

questions emerge: what is the role of MW in learning processes? What does it

happen during MW in classroom? How does it happen and to whom? Can MW

be part of teaching-learning processes, as the other mental functions? How it is

experienced by teachers and students? The field of mind-wandering in

educa-tional contexts becomes so fruitful and open to explorations.

Among the different questions, that we have just begun to address in a

previ-ous work (Dario & Tateo, 2019), this article focuses in particular on the study of

teachers’ MW during their lessons, and on how it relates to their perspective on

students’ experience. The purpose is to understand the teachers’ experience of

MW in classroom. The research questions concern the perception of the MW and

the similarities and dissimilarities between teachers’ and students’ experience.

The empirical study is based on an adaptation of the protocol to study MW

(Dario & Tateo, 2019)

2

. The approach described in the following section is based

on: data collecting in an ecological setting, first person prospective approach

to-ward teachers’ narration, the idea of studying teacher mind-wandering in its

de-velopment and the co-generation of meanings considering both participants’ and

researchers’ interpretation of this psychic phenomenon (Dario & Tateo, 2019).

2. Methodology

We developed a specific empirical protocol using a mix of methods that would

capture both the qualitative and the quantitative attributes of MW situations in

the classroom (Dario & Tateo, 2019). For this sake, we chose to observe different

types of activities in different school grades. Aware of the heterogeneity of

teach-ing activity, we limited the fields to three learnteach-ing situations: lecturteach-ing, cooperative

learning and testing. We asked teachers to describe their lessons in details in

order to better define the characteristics of the teacher and teaching

methodolo-gies. The focus of this protocol is on teachers’ MW narrations that we analysis

using Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis (IPA).

2.1 Participants and Design

The observations discussed in the next section are part of a larger study on 103

Italian students of Primary, Middle and High School, aged between 8 and 18. For

the sake of this article, 7 tenured teachers involved in the study on a voluntary

basis are presented. The teachers (2 from primary, 2 from middle and 3 from high

school respectively) participated in a kick-off meeting to discuss the protocol and

to complete a questionnaire about the learning activities. They were

video-recorded with their students and they were interviewed on their MW experience,

at the end of their lesson, during a break or in a moment of rest. The students

have been selected because they were cited by their teachers during the interview.

Thus, we decided to insert them to further develop and enrich the analysis (e.g.

2 The rationale and the phases of the protocol are detailed in Dario &Tateo ( 2019) and in a form that makes easy to replicate the study.

(5)

“X certainly was mind-wandering when because…”; “He always wanders during

school lessons”, “he considers himself inept”).

2.2 Observation protocol

The protocol used in the present study is adapted from Dario & Tateo (2019)’s

structure, applying it to teachers and to one or two students, who were cited

dur-ing the interviewdur-ing phase.

A quite simple observation protocol was constructed, using a mix of

observa-tions, interviews and teachers researchers’ joint interpretations. The protocol

con-sisted of:

a brief questionnaire, completed by teachers before each lesson in which they

stated the contents and the methodologies used.

a 15/20-minute video-recorded lesson in which the students and teachers

marked mind wandering by inserting a small cotton ball in a plastic cup.

an in-depth and a semi-structured interview

3

conducted with teachers and the

selected students. Interviews revolved around MW experience participants

had during the recorded lesson and also while they were watching it on the

computer.

The protocol is built to include a description of the whole teaching/learning

process, and to grasp the perspective of the teachers involved considering

stu-dents and researchers, too.

2.3 Data Collection protocol

We have tried to develop an observation protocol that is sensitive to both the

per-sonal meaning-making of teachers and the ecological conditions in which the MW

episodes emerge and fade away (Dario &Tateo, 2019). The protocol contains the

phases illustrated in figure 1. Here, we give a brief presentation and we highlight

changes that we made.

3 The choice of the semi-structured interview form is based on the need of a script to guide the in-terview; prepare material to facilitate the recording of the dialogue and systematize the interview in a textual form. This process of constructing texts creates a logical structure for each response that in turn facilitates data analysis. In this regard, the current study has been conducted using semi-structured interview but the researchers prepared the questions in accordance with principles such as: clarity, a series of open and focused questions, avoiding instructions and arranging questions logically (Ergenekon, 2007).

(6)

Figure 1. illustrates the different methodological phases

First, we established a collaboration with the school, meeting the principal and

the teacher interested in participating to the study. On this occasion, the

re-searcher had the opportunity to clarify and answer all questions. In phase 1, the

teacher scheduled times and activities that will be video-recorded, and a brief

questionnaire was submitted to describe the planned teaching/learning activities

that will be observed.

During the instruction phase (phase 2), the researcher presented the study

and the task to both teachers and students: they’ll have to signal their MW by

putting a cotton ball in the container on their desk every time they realize they

(7)

are having a MW experience (only students were given a limited number of ball

while teacher received more than 20 balls each).

However, the teacher and students who needed more balls could indicate

it/the numbers on the sheet of paper that they received at the end of

section/les-son (15/20 minute). The researcher monitored this phase, in order to check

whether the teachers understood the task. Participants asked questions and

re-ceived answers. Then the activity was video-recorded (phase 3). During their free

time, teachers were interviewed

4

.

The researcher ensured participant’s responses anonymity. Each teacher’s

in-terview (video-recorded) was divided in two parts. First, we asked students about

their experience and what they remembered about MW episodes. Then, they

could watch some of the video-recorded moments of the activity in which they

put marbles in the container and were asked about those moments, the transitions

in and out MW episodes, the contents of the episodes and the modalities (visual,

imageless, auditory, etc.). The conversation was terminated when we found an

an-swer for each main theme, with a step by step interview.

Afterwards, the researcher watched the video and then conducted the

partic-ipants to a different room, where she gave them an introductory speech and

in-formed them that their opinions would be used for scientific purposes. These

interviews lasted 30/40 minutes each.

The video-recording of the classroom activity is used as a boundary object, a

mediation tool through which student and researcher construct a shared

under-standing of the student first-person account. It is not a “real time” access to MW,

but it is surely a step forward the observation of the phenomenon in its context.

2.4 IPA Approach to Data Analysis Phase

The interviews with teachers are analyzed with IPA theoretical orientation (Smith

& Shinebourne, 2012), in which persons make sense of their experiences and are

considered “self-interpreting being”, actively engaged in interpreting the

pro-cesses, events and people in their lives (Appendix 1). In the first place, IPA

ap-proach is a dynamic process in which both the researcher and the participant have

an interpretative role, and they evaluate the private nature of mind-wandering.

The step-forward with respect to the previous studies is to examine the individual

perspective before inductively formulating a hypothesis. Each teacher is

consid-ered as a single case, a specimen of generalizable theoretical dimensions of MW

(Valsiner, 2017). The primary concern of IPA approach is to elicit rich, detailed and

first-person accounts of experience and phenomena under investigation and it is

connected with semi-structured, in-depth, one-on-one interview where

re-searcher and participants are engaged in a dialogue in real time.

According to this approach, researchers totally immerged themselves in the

data and give evidence of the participants’ making sense of phenomena under

4 The researcher gave the teachers and students an informative message and remained in the cla-ssroom to answer questions: “All you have to do during this lesson, is signal by inserting a marble into a plastic cup when you notice that your mind is beginning to wander, you are not completely focused on the task at hand and / or you are thinking about something else. Don’t worry, there is nothing strange or unusual about all this; in fact, it happens about 4000 times a day. The purpose of the research you are participating in is to understand its usefulness in education. I remind you that the entire procedure is video-recorded and will be commented upon with those of you that will be interviewed”

(8)

analysis. In Appendix 1 is presented an example of an interview to one of the

teachers, where we conducted:

a multiple reading of the transcript,

a transformation of notes into emergent themes,

a research of relationships.

3. Results

All teachers have been interviewed after each of the three learning activities:

lec-turing, cooperative learning, testing, so the material was very rich and the full

dis-cussion would exceed the space of a single article. We will here discuss the

themes that are in the foreground in teachers’ reflections Teachers reported a

per-sonal interpretation of the MW phenomenon and of the implications for

learn-ing-teaching situations. All interviews presented a similar pattern, but differences

emerged in teachers’ interpretation of the phenomenon.

We reported the analysis divided in three parts: teachers’ experience of

mind-wandering, similarities and dissimilarities between teachers and students and

teachers’ reflexivity. As explained above, the majority of the participants expressed

an opinion or described how the students appeared during the protocol.

3.1 Teachers’ experience of mind-wandering

The teachers always started by presenting the class perception: “All students

seemed calm”, “at the beginning, they were aware of the presence of the camera

but then…”, “This activity generated in my students this…” It is worth noting the

reference to a normality in classroom atmosphere that the teacher uses as a point

of reference in evaluating the events

T15: Before starting, I thought it would be worse: of being

under…(observa-tion), you know, under…so judged as teachers by you …but then I noticed the students…they haven’t done anything differently from the daily routine. At the beginning, the first time I saw that they were a bit (nervous)…but then they were calmed down.

T1: No, no, also today those who normally pay attention, did so… those who are usually distracted, didn’t behave differently…. And then there are those who didn’t insert (cotton) balls but there were many who were distracted. T5: […] but the children because it is a novelty for a brief period are aware of its presence but after a while they don’t even notice it anymore. During the activity, maybe it was Robert who was distracted, during the activity… and he told me that the computer was dying out…you see, during the lesson, I was thinking precisely of him (student) him that I don’t understand… I: What does it mean?

5 Teachers’ and students’ opinions were cited by coding them without using their names in accor-dance with privacy policy. In this regard, individuals were respectively coded and numbered T and P and a progressive number. All names reported are fictional.

(9)

T5: that he often seems lost, wandering, that he stares into something but then he comes out with a pertinent answer… pertinent… he has always had this halo of mystery, let’s say… he seems a distracted child but actually he follows and he is also sharp in his contributions. Consider that in his first class I asked for (psychological) counselling for him because he didn’t talk and so I asked for counselling…It turned out that he is anorthographic6but

has no difficulties to understanding… but he never paid attention?!

The folk normative theories (Olson & Bruner, 1996) about student

develop-ment, learning, attention, education and behavior constitute the interpretive

framework for the personal conducts of students, leading to an

overrepresenta-tion of learning problems or disruptive behaviors. The teachers’ interpretive

framework – that values control, attention, focus on task, and on teacher’s speech

in classroom - contributes to the negative evaluation of MW experiences as

dis-tractions.

The teachers thus associate mind wandering with distraction and low students’

performance trying to find examples that illustrate this, as reported by T1 and T5.

3.2 Similarities and dissimilarities between teachers and students

Teachers distance themselves from mind-wandering, by affirming that this could

never happen to a teacher because she has no time for it. Yet, they are

embar-rassed of their own MW when it occurs.

I: Does it happen at school?

T1: No, because I have one lesson after another.

T2: Let’s say it quite difficult in my opinion, for a teacher to get lost in his own world if he is standing. If you lose control it’s the end of it but if move around among them…If you teach directly from a book, it is easier to lose control …I have to control them because otherwise their minds start wandering out of boredom, especially when they have the sensation that they are not being monitored.

I: What emotion do you feel? Is there an emotion?

T3: Yes, there is a bit of disappointment because I realize that different thoughts (MW) interfere with what I want to do at that moment and there is an unproductive interference because I go back to these thoughts and they interfere with the production and since I am in class…I keep an eye on what I’m reading and one on the class because it is not being at home… It is source of distraction. I end up I combine little because when I tell myself: “Now, they are doing the test on their own, because they are old enough I’ll correct the other class’s test but…this only happens a third of the time be-cause… between keeping an eye on the class, correcting the test and day-dreaming…. This bothers me a little.

I: As a teacher, have you ever thought of something different from what you are actually doing?

T5: Yep, yep… I realize that I get distracted but I would know why. This hap-pens when I lose track of what I’m reading and I have to go back but I can

6 As synomimous of agraphia, it indicates a loss of the ability to write or to express thoughts in writing generally because of a brain lesion.

(10)

also have casual thought…or thoughts about things that really worry me. […]

I: Can’t you remember the content?

T5: No, I can’t recall the content but it (MW) happened for sure … after all that happened (she is referring to the outbreak that hit the school) at school, I usually think: “if all this has happened to me…imagine the children!”.

T2 emphasized the point that lab work in which students were involved in

groups hasn’t run into any glitch and doesn’t allow any mind-wandering because

it is too hectic for him. He underlines how this type of work required the teacher

to be always present, to be in here and now. He is distressed when he realizes

that some of his students (half of them were not doing anything during his class),

and he blames himself for having given them the opportunity to get distracted.

The normative frame leads the teacher (T5) to interpret any shift of focus as a

prob-lematic situation, even her own MW.

I: Have you thought anything else during this lesson?

T2: Not really. Let’s say that this type of lab it is very demanding and they have to work with pictures, with the chronometer, and they have to time the dying/coloration process. Maybe, when we were at a third through the lesson I thought I could have prepared twice as many kits in order to involved the other half of the class but…the problem is that if you don’t have a technician that help you, you can only follow half of the class... till they become inde-pendent but in a month or two, they will be able to work simultaneously… This is normal because they are not self-sufficient enough.

[…]

T2: I thought I could have prepared one kit for group so that they could have work simultaneously as you can see here…they surely have thought about something else.

I: Did you get this idea because something happened?

T2: No, it came back…I had the same thought before…since in this case we use acids, solutions so that I can leave them alone… They have to be fol-lowed…You can see (in the video) that they asked for help. we shall see… They think about something else however if they had been in (groups) of three peoples I sure I could have motivated them…You see (in the video) one can prepare a report, one can wear the lab coat and another one can help in the preparation but the third person it is almost superfluous ..he doesn’t know where to place himself. If they have a rule, it is unlikely that they get distracted.

I: Is there something in this situation that triggered the thought?

T2: No…You see when a person is in the lab he uses a highly concentrated acid…and I also have to make sure that they walk around with pipette…I have no time to think about something else…been in charge of four groups of four people each it is difficult enough.

you can also see from the video that they are calling me at the same time and I am a bit distressed by the fact that they get stuck and I think about it… I: So you have a number of thoughts one after the other?

T2: Yes…that’s way in my lab I need an experienced technician.

For example, I noticed that some students were at the far end of the class-room and they were not participating and call one of them trying to getting involved.

T2 describes an interesting movement between the here and now and the

im-mediate future. She acknowledges it as problematic, but we may suggest a

differ-ent interpretation. T2 is involved in a continuous as-if/as-is elaboration (“I could

(11)

have”; “they have to”; “it came back”; etc.) that links the previous choices to the

monitoring of the ongoing activity and the exploration of alternatives.

In the previous example, also T3 and T5 describe disengagement from the task

as problematic distraction, like in the case of students. The teacher engages in too

many off-task thoughts while reading, which reduces her comprehension. As

Franklin, Smallwood and Schooler (2011) McVay and Kane (2012) and Smallwood,

McSpadden and Schooler (2008) confirm, success in school depends on reading

comprehension, thereby reducing the amount of time spent in off-task thought

would be one way of improving academic performance. This is one of the main

aspect stressed by research that has concentrated its interest on these aspects:

reading/lecturing comprehension. However, T5 explains this phenomenon in

other terms: “While analyzing the test (testing), they are more concentrated on

what they are doing but reading it is more of an automatic action for them. If I

read aloud, I don’t always know what I’m reading it is as I separated the two

things”. Furthermore, T5 describes the perception of blank thoughts connected

to this “automatic reading” and the painful experiences of mind-wandering

with-out contents. She is embarrassed for not remembering part of the activity, too.

The influence of “re-reading” experience in students and teachers is explained

by Philips et al. (2016): when you read over and over again the same text, deliberate

mind-wandering increases due to lower demands in executive control. In a sense,

if teacher T5 has to test reading skills in her pupils, this protracted activity will

leave children free to wander. All teachers connected MW with personal

experi-ence of boredom and repetitive actions. Teachers indicated Teachers’ Refresh

Courses as a common situation when they wander (T1: [...] But, if I can safely say…

when we attend a training course…Some courses are so boring…I wander…So,

everyone wanders with his mind). T1 finds that similar activities in our daily life

are: attending to mass and ironing.

I: Have you ever happened to have any?

T1: Yes many…during Mass… but during the sermon, I mean… the first five minutes, maybe yes (referring to her attention span),… then if it is interesting maybe even ten minutes otherwise….I have many (referring to MW). I: Is there a recurring mind-wandering?

T1: Ah…when i put in in my head to do a chore at home, I keep hearing a voice that every second says to me: “Lucia, you must do that chore …but I swear… A times I invent chores like for two consecutive times, I have reno-vated the house window, I mean, it a men’s job. In such a hard job, but if I put it into my head to do something… I may be talking to you but I’m really thinking about that job. It is like when you wake up in the morning and a tune pops into your head and it stays there the whole day long.

I: When you have a mind-wandering episode? Do you know where you are? Do you know what’s happing around you?

T1: In my case, for instance, It happens while ironing and the I start thinking and then I really find myself somewhere else. I’m in the basement ironing… It’s just me and Lilly (her dog) but if the bell rings, I don’t ever hear it. Perhaps, when I was listening to the sermons, I didn’t notice if the lady next to me, turned or you look but you only see the emptiness…You look into the emptiness…

In the examples above, one can appreciate the role of the context in the

mean-ing-making of MW experience. Depending on the context of activity (class, mass,

chores) the experience takes a different value and different justifications are

(12)

elab-orated. Besides, the excerpts show the relationship between inner generated

thought in MW and environmental conditions. There seems to be no mutual

ex-clusion (MW as escape from reality), rather a mutual feeding of one process into

the other.

3.3 Teachers’ reflexivity

Making sense of both teachers’ and students’ MW in classroom opens a reflexive

space for the teacher. Discussing the way teachers understand students’ MW in

the previous section, we have seen how they recognize similar conditions for the

phenomenon to occur and attribute negative value to it. MW is not just a students’

things, it is a universal phenomenon. By acknowledging this, teachers become

aware, during the interviews, that MW signals some critical moment in

teaching/learning process. Teachers know exactly when their students are

inat-tentive and start mind-wandering, because they perfectly identify the boring and

repetitive lecture parts.

T1: …but more than one (student) was…distracted. I: Who, for example…?

T1: For example, I kept an eye on P.43…he didn’t do anything. But he told me that he doesn’t understand the subject to begin with.

P.5: […] I saw myself inside of me because I wasn’t thinking about the test anymore… I didn’t give a damn about it.

I put it on (the desk) and I stayed there on the chair and I remained there to fantasize…in short, alone with my thoughts.

Researcher and interviewee P.08 are in front of the video and we hear T2 that

states: «the mitochondrion is a small organ…».

T2, when the lesson contains reiterated elements, is conscious of the

possibil-ity of students’ mind-wandering.

T2: After few minutes, I noticed that they were beginning to get distracted… I have already told about mitochondria and so when I told about them again… some of the students started thinking about something else… For this reason, I even slowed down…and I thought about that (slowing down) P.08: Here it is! … (the student, watching the video with researcher points to the professor statement the moment he has a mind-wandering activity). P.09: It is as if I heard a voice that…

I: When did you think you heard this voice?

P.09: When he talked about the structure of the eukaryotes cell and prokary-otes cell.

I: Excuse me, this is the subject of lesson… The precise moment instead? P.09: Ah, while he was talking about mitochondria, I think...

P.10: It is as if the teacher put the mute mode and then when He says the things that you want to talk about you start concentrated on the lesson again…Ones again you heard the teacher’s words.

I: Was there a moment in which that happens?

P.10: Yes, right at the beginning… when he was talking about what he had al-ready explain…

P.11… The teacher mentioned mitochondria and an acronym and I saw a bolt in the chair in front of me (this reminds him of a film). A scene of the film in

(13)

which you can see the bolt entering a wall …but also the lesson does not in-terest me.

P6 claims that his teacher (T5) arrives when he is intent on deciding which

sub-ject to choose from. So reading it again with her was useful but T5 says that she

saw him spell bound. The idea that a spell bound person should be blocked and

brought back to his task is very common. According to T2, a way of avoiding MW

consists of constant control and monitoring strategies. For him, the teacher’s

pos-ture (standing) and the kind of task influence the way students pay attention. T2,

for example, in addition to lecturing, also hands out a summary sheet to be filled

out by the students. This makes mind-wandering more difficult because of the

demand made by multitasking: the student has to listen and, at the same time, to

fill in the schedule. Students interviewed confirmed this teacher’s view (Do you

feel that you were thinking about something else during the lesson? P2a: I don’t

think one can be inattentive during the professor’s lesson (T2)). In this manner,

students are always involved but, as in Teachers’ Refresher Courses the possibility

of deliberate mind-wandering is still there as confirmed by students. P2b is not

motivated toward the discipline and says that he decided to stare at the window

and think about his problems (P2b: I thought about all my problems, as I told you.

He knows (referring to T2) that I’m not interested in his lesson; P1: […] I saw myself

inside of me because I wasn’t thinking about the test anymore … I didn’t give a

damn about it. I put it on (the desk) and I stayed there on the chair and I remained

there to fantasize…in short, alone with my thoughts).

However, all teachers think it is normal to be distracted in a test situation

be-cause they are less involved and there is a passive presence. In this manner, the

concept of testing as a not learning moment but a students’ evaluation emerges.

I: What about today?

T1: Yes, because I was thinking of how to organize the lesson with the stu-dents in mechanics class and, I wasn’t concentrating on them.

When I call on them, no… because I write down what they say and the mis-takes they make. I can’t stop them because otherwise they lose the train of thought and so, I wait for the end of the interview and I tell them what mis-takes they have made.

T6: In my lecturing, I didn’t mind-wander but other times I did (referring to testing and cooperative learning)

Not only, testing is a moment in which teachers believe having no possibilities

to change the present situation (e.g. they cannot move around or go out and they

have to keep quiet and make sure that students are quiet too) and they

mind-wan-der.

In all learning situations, teachers declare to plan future actions during this

ac-tivity in the classroom. They could concern autobiographical or teaching planning

thoughts.

[..] T1: I could already see the students divided into groups and I was thinking about whom to place where, what they had to do…I saw the people... I saw the actual people.

[..]I didn’t see myself but I knew I was there and I communicated with them. And It was also clear (she knows) what behavior they could had had, the an-swers and what I must say when they behave that way because I know them and I know their reaction is going to be and so I also prepared my answers in advance.

(14)

T5: I had thought a table contents about data on productive sectors but then I want to get their opinions, listen to their questions… Even if I could have worked with fellow colleague (the math teacher). That’s what I thought! I didn’t have many (mind-wanderings) thought because I had to concentrate on the questions”.

T3: Yes, because Saturday is a special day because the week is coming and I often read and then I have to do other things…I often think about thing that I should do during the week. But then, in realty I’m not able to concentrate on them because of the class and then when I hear a noise or something, because after all I’m there. On the other hand, I think about my outlook on personal issues and about the daily organization when I am at home. I: Today, did you happen to think about something else?

T3: Yes…I was thinking about the move, it not something that I’m about to do but I think…the baby where am I going to put him? How can I do it? Stuff like that…Actually, I try to find the best solution and so I go back to it often.

The normative frame here has to do with the role owner in the

teaching-learn-ing situation. MW is interpreted in different ways, dependteaching-learn-ing on the person

in-volved. It seems that it is more acceptable for a teacher and less acceptable for a

student.

In T7’s narration, we find that mind wandering is a recursive thought expressed

in images that concerns her and reappears during the lesson: her passion for

an-tiques.

T7 “You see I often see images related my interest… I deal in modern an-tiques and truth anan-tiques and others objects…and at times I happen to think about those things. If I’m deeply involved in a deal, it comes to mind and so I say: “Today, he should come…” [...] “I have to send out and email to so and so… I have to send a picture of this object… things like that…”

For T7, MW is something that has not connection with the contest, it happens

like “when you plan your daily activities… so when you have done it, you don’t

think about any more”.

I: Is emotional aspect involved in it?

T7: Yes, absolutely… thinking about these things relaxes me, they are my pas-sion…I tell you being able to think about these things is relaxing.

T7 treats MW as a restless and re-generative moment but it isn’t considered a

recursive phenomenon, it is not a rumination of past sad events. For her,

mind-wandering is not kinked to tiredness or boredom. She says: “the more things I do,

the more things come to mind”.

During a test where she claims to be thinking about other things, she says she

is planning what to design for the end-of-the-year recital.

T7: “I didn’t draw the toilet bowl because I thought: “If I do it, I’m going to distract them” and so took care of operation I was doing and …but there was a series of thing that I have in mind to do…The I thought how to make the theatre set”.

I: Is there something specific that trigger it?

T7: No. it was among todays’ plans… it is just that then it started again when I was fiddling with todays’ operations. It is a thought inside another thought (parallel) and then when everything was ready I thought of starting to work with them.

(15)

It was already planned out and while I was doing the various operations, I was drawing two numeric lines, … I was thinking about what to do… and how to do it came to me at that moment during the test.

I: Are you bored or active? T7: For me? I’m always active….

T7 always maintains the perception of herself and other around her. In her

opinion the MW is a process that involved images and she sees herself acting in

the first person.

These different MW’s features and modalities, illustrated in these paragraph,

show the heterogeneity of the phenomenon in individuals. Mind –wandering

be-comes an umbrella term for a large varieties of mental process, among which we

find planning, the controfactual process connected specifically to emotional states

and future affective reactions.

T4: Here (showing the video), while I was handing out the tests, P9 said: “Luckily, it is the last one (test)!” and I answered: “Luckily, it is the last one for me too… because I’m the one who has to correct them…and then I had these texts because a girl has asked me if I could read them. I realized that I was not reading them… My student (p9)’s remark (Luckily, it is the last one!) echoed in my head and so I lost contact with the text that I was reading and for a while, I didn’t know where I was.

[…] There was a moment in which I got distracted and I initiated an internal train of thought. You know, I thought that with the other teacher they felt quite free, they kind of did what they wanted and there was a moment in which I got distracted and I thought…You think about those thing because it is always a bit difficult to be oneself. Because in any case, students would always prefer to have fun, to play and they are more comfortable with teacher that are that way…Instead you want them to learn something…You start thinking that maybe you are wrong and one can learn even if sur-rounded by confusion.

I: Do you lose perception of the class?

T4: Yes, a bit because you add things up…your strict side and your more per-missive one to find a balance.

Referring to the protocol, all teachers noticed/stressed that the presence of

the camera was an element that reduced their spontaneity and that of the

stu-dents. For this reason, the camera was introduced 15 minutes before the

video-recording activity and, as confirm by teachers. After few minutes, students seemed

to forget it was there. The other aspect is the number of cotton balls given to

stu-dents in order to report MW: three/four to each of them. The number of

Mind-wanderings in fact could be higher but this shortcoming is overcome by giving

students a sheet to fill in which they record the exact number.

Conclusions

Mind-wandering is an emerging research area in cognitive studies as evidenced

by the trend of publications in recent years but it is viewed as a phenomenon at

the margin of the pedagogical horizon. Thus, there is a paucity of pedagogical

publications on the subject even if virtually all teachers have probably observed

students while they are partaking in an “absent-minded job as if they have stopped

in time” (description of primary school pupils during a focus group).

(16)

Basing our observation on a dynamic idea of learning and teaching processes,

in this paper we analyze what happens to teachers, confirming similarities with

students’ MW (Dario & Tateo, 2019).

We are able to confirm that teachers’ and students’ attitude towards MW is

negative according to the equation: attention-learning-high performance and

inat-tention-disorder-not learning/low performance. This evaluation is guided by the

normative frame of reference of the school, through which the actor involved

make sense of a psychic experience that seems to deviate from the expected

con-ducts.

As a consequence, MWs are seen as an intimate affair, hence teachers are less

comfortable describing them to other people, because they have experienced the

limits of social tolerance of them. In particular, the peculiar negative research

ap-proach towards MW pushed teachers to adopt strategies to limit/reduce it. In this

manner, mind-wandering is a phenomenon relegated to the margin in the same

way in which mistakes were seen in past decades. In fact, for a long time, teachers

took a dim view of mistakes but now they tend to use them as a tool to improve

teaching-learning actions.

By looking for and uncovering our errors, our mistakes, our inadequacies, and then eliminating them. This is how we improve our practical knowl-edge—our writing skills, our ability to draw, our tennis game—and how we improve our knowledge of the world and our understanding of other people. We are fallible, so we make mistakes. But we can learn from our mistakes. By uncovering and eliminating them, we improve our knowledge and under-standing (Perkinson, 2002)

However, teachers accept their MWs during testing and group activities

be-cause they consider these activities as students’ experiences where they are not

personally involved. In most cases, as shown by available data, teachers’ work is

closely related to lecturing.

Furthermore, MW always shows:

teachers and students consider repetitive activities and boredom a cause of

MW (Singer, 1974; Philips et al. 2016), in particular, deliberate MW that could

occur at the same moments in students and teachers. In this case, they prefer

staying in another world instead of being in the real one;

the teachers who declare to be active during mind-wandering, report feeling

active after a MW episode: a cognitive jumpstart, this shift from here to there

“revitalizes” the mind. Subjects are involved in a restless moment that is

capa-ble of improving their future performances and results.

According to these two statements, re-reading the same text or not being

ac-tively involved in a situation (the teacher that has to monitor his students during

the test) move towards maladaptive mind-wandering that reduces personal

in-volvement in the real world, but also some educational questions arise: does every

educational activity where there is “re” (reading, writing, listening,

re-drawing) favors mind-wandering? Could MW be a way of giving relevance and

meaningfulness to boring and senseless individual or collective situations? Or, if

MW is a common phenomenon in psychic life, what is its positive function?

In addition, the above cited interpretative analysis suggests that MW

corre-sponds to a restless moment that permits conceptual elaborations and the

emer-gence of affective reactions to solve states of feelings. In this manner, we question

(17)

the concept of relevance and meaningfulness which are directly linked to

self-es-teem and previous experiences, but also with semantic interaction with reality.

MW, as an imaginative process, reminds us how, since the time of Aristoteles,

imagination has played a role in memory formation and retrieval and association

of ideas. Since the time of the teachers of rhetoric, such as Cicero and Quintillian,

new relationships has been created between experiences and new information,

generating interactive images between old ideas and new events. During

mind-wanderings, teachers monitor their behaviours towards others: teachers planning

their actions and relationships with students. Thus, they generate a hypothesis on

how to organize their future lessons and imagine students’ reactions. There is a

sort of future prefiguration moved by internal states (Tateo, 2016). In fact, MW

fea-tures scenarios of future goal-related activities: an important goal may spawn

hun-dreds or thousands of brief MWs of goal-related actions (Klinger, 2009). Teachers

report that recursive issues or purposes generate recursive MW and rumination

but the experience of rumination is rarely linked to unhappy feelings. In teachers’

mind, rumination is connected to pertinence and personality, as expressed by

Singer (1966). In conclusion, as in the case of mistake, MW is an event that

hap-pens and transforms our learning experience, in particular when students have

to reach a performative result. MW could be included in those psychic

phenom-ena of imaginative nature (Brown & Tateo, 2019; Tateo, 2016) which seem to play a

fundamental role in all future-oriented cognitive and affective processes, which

require a systematic exploration in ecological contexts of the activity. However,

the above assertions are not an attempt to justify distraction but consider MW

one of the language known by the subject: thinking in terms of mind-wandering

means to facilitate the re-enaction o simulation processes useful for the

develop-ment of concepts and language. (Clowes e Mendonça, 2016; Goldman, 2005).

We know, for example as without an erratic movement of mind, no teacher

would be able to understand the meaning of the intentionally incorrect sentences:

“The Boas their prey in the jungle”, moving from an external online reality (the

written sentence) to an inner state able to retrieve previous reading experiences.

Hence further questions arise rather than conclusions because only a free

mind, which does not jump to conclusions can be considered non-mechanical

(Krishnamurti, 2010):

How to use autobiographical narrations (teachers and pupils) to understand

how the lesson, we are imparting as teachers, is interconnected with the

af-fective and relational levels of subjects and how the organization of the action

changes in its making? Mind-wandering shows us that we must teach students

to ask themselves: Am I forcing myself to learn these things, while I would like

to do something else? Where is my interest going while I’m here in this

learn-ing situation that doesn’t involve me? What would I like to learn? How would

I like to learn it? How am I restructuring the information, according to personal

and original methods?

How to indulge the erratic mind with practices that include “the method of

loci” (attribution of concepts to very different places that ancient cultures used

to memorize and to indulge the wandering mind: it means to associate a

men-tal path, for example planning, to physical places) or doodling (spontaneous

form of writing that redirects MW towards an organized form and favors deep

learning processes because it combines the emotional states of the subject

with concepts) transforming MW from useless to useful for learning?

(18)

In essence, we find it absurd not to consider mind-wandering or even worse

to want to eliminate it at all costs because it is a part of our mental processes and

of our imagination. Certainly it must be blocked when maladaptive (Somer, 2009)

but studied and understood when it is a process of interpretation and

manage-ment and a way of evaluating unexplored perspectives of our life experiences.

References

Antrobus, J. S., Singer, J. L., & Greenberg, S. (1966). Studies in the stream of consciousness: experimental enhancement and suppression of spontaneous cognitive processes.

Per-ceptual and Motor Skills, 23(2), 399-417.

Bateson, G. (1972). The logical categories of learning and communication. Steps to an Ecol-ogy of Mind, 279-308.

Bronfenbrenner U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press.

Brown, S. & Tateo, L. (eds.) (2019). The method of imagination. Charlotte, NC: Information Age.

Dario, N., & Tateo, L. (2019). A New Methodology for the Study of Mind-Wandering Pro-cess. Human Arenas, 3(2), 172-189.

Clowes, R. W., & Mendonca, D. (2016). Representation Redux: Is there still a useful role for representation to play in the context of embodied, dynamicist and situated theories of mind? New ideas in psychology, 40, 26-47.

Fossa, P., Gonzalez, N., & Di Montezemolo, F. C. (2019). From inner speech to mind-wander-ing: developing a comprehensive model of inner mental activity trajectories. Integrative

Psychological and Behavioral Science, 53(2), 298-322.

Franklin, M. S., Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2011). Catching the mind in flight: Using behavioral indices to detect mindless reading in real time. Psychonomic bulletin &

re-view, 18(5), 992-997.

Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston, MA, US.

Goldman, A. (2005). Imitation, mind reading, and simulation. Perspectives on imitation: From

neuroscience to social science, 2, 79-93.

Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind.

Sci-ence, 330(6006), 932-932.

Klinger, E. (2009). Daydreaming and fantasizing: Thought flow and motivation. In K. D. Mark-man, W. M. P. Klein, & J. A. Suhr (Eds.), Handbook of imagination and mental

simula-tion (pp. 225-239). New York, NY, US: Psychology Press.

Krishnamurti, J. (2010). Freedom from the Known. Random House.

Irving, Z., & Thompson, E. (2018). The philosophy of mind-wandering. In The Oxford

Hand-book of Spontaneous Thought: Mind-Wandering, Creativity, and Dreaming (pp. 87-96).

Oxford University Press.

Jha, A. P., Morrison, A. B., Dainer-Best, J., Parker, S., Rostrup, N., & Stanley, E. A. (2015). Minds “at attention”: Mindfulness training curbs attentional lapses in military cohorts. PloS

one, 10(2), e0116889.

Lindquist, S. I., & McLean, J. P. (2011). Daydreaming and its correlates in an educational en-vironment. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(2), 158-167.

Olson, D. R., & Bruner, J. S. (1996). Folk psychology and folk pedagogy. In D. S. Olson & N. Torrance (Eds), The handbook of education and human development: New models of

learning, teaching and schooling (pp. 9-27). Oxford: Blackwell Publishing

Phillips, N. E., Mills, C., D’Mello, S., & Risko, E. F. (2016). On the influence of re-reading on mind wandering. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(12), 2338-2357. Martin, L., Mills, C., D’mello, S. K., & Risko, E. F. (2018). Re-watching lectures as a study

strat-egy and its effect on mind wandering. Experimental psychology.

McMillan, R., Kaufman, S. B., & Singer, J. L. (2013). Ode to positive constructive daydream-ing. Frontiers in psychology, 4, 626.

(19)

Regis, M. (2013). Daydreams and the Function of Fantasy. Springer.

McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2012). Why does working memory capacity predict variation in reading comprehension? On the influence of mind wandering and executive attention. Journal of experimental psychology: general, 141(2), 302.

Regis, M. (2013). Daydreams and the Function of Fantasy. Springer.

Seli, P., Beaty, R. E., Marty-Dugas, J., & Smilek, D. (2019). Depression, anxiety, and stress and the distinction between intentional and unintentional mind wandering. Psychology of

Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6(2), 163.

Singer, J. L. (1966). Daydreaming: An introduction to the experimental study of inner

expe-rience. Crown Publishing Group/Random House

Singer, J. L. (1974). Daydreaming and the stream of thought. American Scientist.

Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 946– 958.

Smallwood, J., McSpadden, M., & Schooler, J. W. (2008). When attention matters: The curious incident of the wandering mind. Memory & Cognition, 36(6), 1144-1150.

Smallwood, J. (2013b). Distinguishing how from why the mind wanders: a process–occur-rence framework for self-generated mental activity. Psychological bulletin, 139(3), 519. Smith, J. A., & Shinebourne, P. (2012). Interpretative phenomenological analysis. In H.

Cooper, P. M. Camic, D. L. Long, A. T. Panter, D. Rindskopf, & K. J. Sher (Eds.), APA

hand-book of research methods in psychology, Vol. 2. Research designs: Quantitative,

quali-tative, neuropsychological, and biological (pp. 73-82). Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association.

Singer, J. L. (1966). Daydreaming: An introduction to the experimental study of inner

expe-rience.

Somer, Eli (2002). Maladaptive daydreaming: A qualitative inquiry. Journal of Contemporary

Psychotherapy, 32, 197–212.

Szpunar, K. K., Moulton, S. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2013). Mind wandering and education: from the classroom to online learning. Frontiers in psychology, 4, 495.

Tateo, L. (2016). What imagination can teach us about higher mental functions. In Psychology

as the science of human being (pp. 149-164). Springer, Cham.

Valsiner, J. (2017). From methodology to methods in human psychology. New York: Springer Varao-Sousa, T. L., & Kingstone, A. (2015). Memory for lectures: How lecture format impacts

the learning experience. PloS one, 10(11), e0141587.

Wammes, J. D., Ralph, B. C., Mills, C., Bosch, N., Duncan, T. L., & Smilek, D. (2019). Disen-gagement during lectures: Media multitasking and mind wandering in university class-rooms. Computers & Education, 132, 76-89.

(20)

Appendix 1 – Example of data analysis

Original transcript Exploratory comments Emerging themes

I: What do you think about this protocol? What hap-pened to you during it?

T1: Before starting, I thought it would be worse: of being under…(observation), you know, under…so judged as teachers by you …but then I noticed the students…they haven’t done anything differently from the daily routine. At the beginning, the first time I saw that they were a bit (nervous)…but then they were calmed down.

(silence)

T1: No, no, also today those who normally pay atten-tion, did so… those who are usually distracted, didn’t behave differently…. And then there are those who didn’t insert (cotton) balls but there were many who were distracted.

Katia’s perception of the protocol and interview. She stresses the idea at- tention-learning-perfor-mance and

inattention-disorder-not learning.

There is not difference between distraction and inattention.

Mind-wandering is a form of distraction

Teachers’ common idea: attention-learning-perfor-mance and inattention-disorder-not learning.

Referring to Mind wan-dering as a phenomenon to be reduced

I: Could you explain me that?

T1: For example, I kept an eye on F.D…he didn’t do anything. But he told me that he doesn’t understand the subject to begin with.

Read F. D.’S interview Example of a student’s deliberate mind-wander-ing motivated by learn-ing-teaching situation

Elements in common be-tween teachers and stu-dents’ narrations. Deliberate mind-wander-ing

T1: Then, B. that he never hasn’t anything with him and always turns in not filling tests. He didn’t insert a cotton ball but he had to insert an entire bag because he didn’t pay attention at a single moment. at one… and I saw the others but they were normal then I don’t know if they were looking at the test but think-ing to someththink-ing else.

Ah…Then there is M. R. who distracted also when he is drawing…You see him living in another world. T1: Then, B. who never does anything, turns in a blank sheet of paper. He didn’t insert a cotton ball while he should have inserted an entire bag because he didn’t pay attention for a single moment… not even one… and the others behaved normally, how-ever I didn’t know if they were looking at the paper were thinking about something else.

Ah…Then there is M. R. who is distracted even when he is drawing…You can see that he is living in his own world.

She reflected on the ne-cessity to give students more balls.

She uses a common ex-planation: living in an-other world

A methodological reflec-tion on the necessity of giving more than three balls to each students Attribute to mind wan-dering the idea of “living in another world”

I: Did you happen to think about something else? T1: Yes, while they were doing the test, I was think-ing about the work with the mechanics course, so I wasn’t paying attention…paying attention on them. But, if I can say, when we attend a training course… Some courses are so boring…I wander…So, everyone wanders with his mind.

I: What about today?

T1: Yes, because I was thinking of how to organize the lesson with the students in mechanics’ class and, I wasn’t concentrating on them.

When I call on them, no… because I write down what they say and the mistakes they make. I can’t stop them because otherwise they lose the train of thought and so, I wait for the end of the interview and I tell them what mistakes they have made.

She associates MW with other life events: Teach-ers’ Refresher Courses and Sunday Mass. This teacher has a delib-erate mind-wandering, during these events.

Boredom and repetitive activities generate mind-wandering episodes and in particular deliberate MW.

(21)

Original transcript Exploratory comments Emerging themes

I: Do you ever happen to have any?

T1: Yes many…during Mass… but during the ser-mon, I mean… the first five minutes, maybe yes (re-ferring to her attention span) …, then it is interesting maybe even ten minutes otherwise… I have many (referring to MW).

I: Is there a recurring mind-wandering?

T1: Ah…when I put in in my head to do a chore at home, I keep hearing a voice that every second says to me: “Katia, you must do that chore …but I swear… A times I invent chores like for two consecutive times, I have renovated the house window, I mean, it a men’s job. In such a hard job, but if I put it into my head to do something…I may be talking to you but I’m really thinking about that job. It is like when you wake up in the morning and a tune pops into your head and it stays there the whole day long. I: Does it happen at school?

T1: No, because i have one lesson after another I: When you have a mind-wandering episode? Do you know where you are? Do you know what’s hap-ping around you?

T1: In my case, for instance, it happens while ironing and then I start thinking and then I really find myself somewhere else. I’m in the basement ironing…It’s just me and Lilly (her dog) but if the bell rings, I don’t ever hear it.

Perhaps, when I was listening to the sermons, I didn’t notice if the lady next to me, turned or you look but you only see the emptiness…You look into the emptiness…

A personal issue or pur-pose generate recursive mind-wandering. T1 refers that MW is not common for a teacher in particular because he is always involved in activi-ties without restless.

She fixed the emptiness

Recursive mind wander-ing is generated by per-sonal issue.

Fixation: sign of MW I: Were you aware of looking in the emptiness?

Yes, I’m starring at the emptiness but If you asked me what kind of coat was the woman in front wearing, I wouldn’t know how to answer. In that moment I was-n’t attentive. But it is normal, iswas-n’t it?

I: Were you aware that you were looking into the emptiness?

T1: Yes, I was looking at people but if you asked me what kind of coat the woman in front was wearing, I wouldn’t have known what to answer. In that moment I wasn’t attentive… But it is quite normal, isn’t it?

Mind-wandering is an example of absentminded person when he lost the perception of what is doing.

Maladaptive mind-wan-dering

I: In your opinion, do you think there is an emotion that has triggered this thought?

T1: Yes, yes I: Maybe the Mass?

T1: Certainly not the Mass. It happened in a period in which I was having some serious problems, and I remember this scene: “I was at the supermarket, at the fish counter and I was looking towards the exit… at the supermarket…and I saw …Then I knew that there were things around me but I was focusing on the supermarket floor and I saw this huge floor that was making me anxious…but something serious had happened at home… so it was some stuff at home, then, I had a terrible feeling, this feeling of just being overwhelmed by things around me. ..I felt that big (she indicates the small size with her hands).. Sur-rounding by these enormous tiles…it didn’t last long…I perceived a sense of powerlessness, of help-lessness...I mean, it was strange because I always have everything under control…(for me) the glass is always half full but at that moment I was beginning to see it half empty.

Because a number of important had piled up…I was scared…at least a little

The teacher has a state of feeling and mind-wan-dering prepared the reac-tion.

Mind-wandering pre-pared the affective reac-tion.

Figura

Figure 1. illustrates the different methodological phases

Riferimenti

Documenti correlati

The article focuses on events of which the authors have been spectators during their research work in Morocco (district of Boukhalef, outskirts of Tangier, August 2014): the

This is due to the fact that, in non-isothermal conditions, the average temperature of the flow is higher than in the isothermal one: this means lower density of the fluid

This article was submitted to Neurotrauma, a section of the journal Frontiers in Neurology Received: 28 September 2017 Accepted: 12 February 2018 Published:

Under the lens of his philosophy and metaphysical thinking, Coleridge and Contempla- tion revolves around Coleridge’s synaesthetic interconnections of soul and mind, music and

This thesis addressed the issue of the mind wandering (MW) considering domains of everyday activities such as driving and narrative discourse production. Many studies have

Moreover, the participants completed the Cognitive Failure Questionnaire (CFQ) and were asked to produce samples of narrative discourse by administering a

CB-PCPs are imidazolium functionalized porous polymers, obtained via facile click-synthesis, and bearing all the common anions used in the field of PILs for carbon

The Oldest Dryas moraine at Mount Pelister marks the youngest glacial feature in this area, suggesting that the wide cirque was not or not significantly re-glaciated during the