• Non ci sono risultati.

Adaptability and plasticity of the human sensory systems

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Condividi "Adaptability and plasticity of the human sensory systems"

Copied!
130
0
0

Testo completo

(1)
(2)
(3)

Contents

Table of figures ... 5

0 General Introduction ... 10

0.1 Recalibrating after disrupting: searching for the lost balance ... 11

0.2 Cross-modal interactions outside of Awareness ... 14

0.3 Recalibrating time perception to self-motion ... 16

0.4 Structure of the thesis ... 18

0.4.1 Abstracts ... 19

1 Early Cross-modal Plasticity in Adults ... 24

1.1 Introduction ... 24

1.2 Methods ... 29

1.2.1 Participants ... 29

1.2.2 Ethic Statement ... 29

1.2.3 Apparatus and Stimuli ... 29

1.2.4 Task and Procedure ... 32

1.2.5 Data Analyses ... 34

1.3 Results ... 36

1.3.1 Main Experiment... 36

1.3.2 Deprivation Control Experiment ... 47

1.4 Discussion ... 49

2 Touch Accelerates Visual Awareness ... 56

2.1 Introduction ... 56

2.2 Materials and Methods ... 61

2.2.1 Participants ... 61

2.2.2 Ethic Statement ... 61

2.2.3 Apparatus and Stimuli ... 61

2.2.4 Experimental Procedure ... 66

(4)

2.2.6 Analyses... 69

2.3 Results ... 70

2.3.1 Main Experiment ... 70

2.3.2 Control Experiments ... 75

2.4 Discussion ... 77

3 Apparent motion causes the time dilation effect ... 84

3.1 Introduction ... 84

3.2 Materials and Methods ... 86

3.2.1 Participants ... 86 3.2.2 Stimuli ... 86 3.2.3 Procedure ... 89 3.2.4 Analyses... 93 3.3 Results ... 93 3.4 Discussion ... 99 4 General Conclusions ... 104 Acknowledgements ... 111 References ... 114

(5)

Table of figures

1. Early Cross-modal Plasticity in Adults

Figure 1.1 __________________________________________________ 31 Figure 1.2 __________________________________________________ 38 Figure 1.3 __________________________________________________ 39 Figure 1.4 __________________________________________________ 43 Figure 1.5 __________________________________________________ 46 Figure 1.6 __________________________________________________ 48

2. Touch Accelerates Visual Awareness

Figure 2.1 __________________________________________________ 63 Figure 2.2 __________________________________________________ 71 Figure 2.3 __________________________________________________ 72 Figure 2.4 __________________________________________________ 73 Figure 2.5 __________________________________________________ 76

3. Apparent motion causes the time dilation effect

Figure 3.1 __________________________________________________ 88 Figure 3.2 __________________________________________________ 95 Figure 3.3 __________________________________________________ 96 Figure 3.4 __________________________________________________ 98 Figure 3.5 __________________________________________________ 99

(6)
(7)

General Abstract (English)

In this work, we present different examples of how the human brain is able to adapt its functionality to different sensory contexts and to a constantly changing world in an attempt to always maximize the efficiency of the information collected and to reduce ambiguities that could arise from the intrinsically vague sensory landscapes we often have to deal with. In our first study, we focus on the known cross-modal plasticity phenomena that occur with anomalous or missing sensory stimulation, when other senses are able to interact with the abnormal modality, with the aim of investigating the timescale and dynamics of these recalibration processes after a short period of monocular deprivation. In a second study, we investigate the extent to which the congruent tactile exploration can help bring to awareness an invisible grating. We make use of a visual stimulation technique that allows us to show that the visuo-haptic interaction occurs outside visual awareness. In a third study, we focus on the temporal dilation effect that occurs on duration judgements for moving objects compared to stationary objects. We make use of recent virtual reality tools to investigate the individual contributions of object motion as opposed to motion produce by the observer’s own

(8)

motion in the environment, showing that the brain makes use of vestibular and proprioceptive signals to distinguish between these two types of motion, and to allow it to adaptively suppress self-generated motion.

Riassunto Generale (Italiano)

In questo lavoro, presentiamo diversi esempi della capacità del cervello umano di adattare la propria funzione a contesti sensoriali differenti e a un mondo in continuo mutamento, capacità che vede le proprie radici nell’esigenza di massimizzare l’efficienza delle informazioni raccolte e di ridurre le ambiguità che possono nascere da scenari sensoriali intrinsecamente vaghi. Nel primo studio, ci siamo concentrati sui ben conosciuti fenomeni di plasticità cross-modale che hanno luogo in caso di stimolazione sensoriale anomala o mancante, grazie ai quali altre modalità sensoriali sono in grado di interagire con la modalità danneggiata, con la finalità di investigare il decorso temporale e la dinamica di questi processi plastici in seguito a deprivazione monoculare a breve termine. In un secondo studio, abbiamo investigato la misura nella

(9)

quale l’esplorazione tattile di un reticolo invisibile sia in grado di riportarlo entro la consapevolezza visiva. Per ciò abbiamo utilizzato una tecnica di stimolazione visiva che permette di mostrare che questa interazione visuo-tattile avviene al di fuori della consapevolezza visiva dell’osservatore. In un terzo studio, ci siamo concentrati sull’effetto di dilatazione temporale che influenza i giudizi di durata temporale per oggetti in movimento rispetto a oggetti immobili. Abbiamo utilizzato recenti metodologie di realtà virtuale per investigare i contributi individuali del movimento degli oggetti e del movimento prodotto dalla locomozione dell’osservatore stesso, mostrando che il cervello utilizza segnali vestibolari e propriocettivi per distinguere tra questi due tipi di movimento e per sopprimere gli effetti del movimento dovuti alla locomozione, in maniera adattativa.

(10)

0 General Introduction

Our senses cover a wide range of physical quantities that we sample through our varied sensory organs, and process through our nervous system to ultimately inform us about our environment. Through this intrinsic variety of types of signals, our nervous system is able to integrate different dimensions that belong to real world objects and to the environment, in order to suppress ambiguities in the representations that might be present within single sensory modalities. This allows us to overcome the limitations of each sensory modality and to maximize the likelihood of detecting interesting events or to escape efficiently from threats. As we will show later in this manuscript, the ability to use this plethora of sources of information can be also used outside of our awareness, making it a partially automatic process.

In this work, we will present three parts in which we investigate different properties of cross-modal integration mechanisms and phenomena.

In our first work, we investigate how our ability to use a haptic signal to help disambiguate a physically ambivalent bi-stable visual

(11)

stimulus can change as a result of a short-term form of cortical plasticity, elicited through short-term visual deprivation.

In a second study, we describe an experiment where tactile exploration can rescue an invisible stimulus from a deep and robust suppression state, bringing it back to the observer’s awareness.

In a last work, we use virtual reality to help us investigate how time estimation of immersive visual stimuli can be warped to accommodate evidence obtained from proprioception and vestibular senses, to help to distinguish between approaching objects and the passively (and illusionary) moving environment when we move within it.

Regardless of the topic being investigated or the tool used, we will show that the way we use all of our senses is extremely flexible and plastic, and can often overcome the boundaries of our physical senses to obtain better information about our world.

0.1 RECALIBRATING AFTER DISRUPTING: SEARCHING FOR THE LOST

BALANCE

The adult human brain retains the ability to change as a function of environmental influences, and it does so to a much higher degree than it

(12)

was thought in the past. In fact, neuroplasticity is now considered to be an intrinsic property of the human brain (Pascual-Leone, Amedi, Fregni, & Merabet, 2005). This susceptibility to external changes peaks during the very first part of our lives, during the critical periods, temporal windows during which the young and still developing brain is able to drastically change in response to any measurable change in the environment (Berardi, Pizzorusso, & Maffei, 2000). For instance, it is known that early blind people show activation in their visual cortex during tactile (i.e. Amedi, Raz, Azulay, Malach, & Zohary, 2010) and auditory (i.e. Collignon et al., 2011) tasks, as a result of massive re-wirings yielding to structural and functional re-colonization of the former visual cortices by other sensory modalities.

Evidence accumulated around the notion that cortical plasticity, considered a staple of our ability to learn new skills and abilities, is retained after the end of critical periods and is responsible for the correct operation of higher functions such as learning and memory (Fuchs & Flugge, 2014).

Evidence hinting at the possibility that plasticity might be retained by adults even for lower-level sensory functions come from works making use of complete and prolonged blindfolding and intensive tactile trainings:

(13)

5 days of blindfolding is able to trigger the transient recruitment of the temporarily-unused visual cortex for tactile processing (Merabet et al., 2008; Kauffman, Theoret, & Pascual-Leone, 2002).

More recently, several lines of work have focused on temporarily disrupting the ocular balance through brief periods of monocular deprivation and then testing the participants with ambiguous visual stimuli such as binocular rivalry. These works show that the adult visual cortex is surprisingly susceptible to changes, with the deprived eye completely and reliably dominating the ocular balance right after deprivation (Lunghi, Burr, & Morrone, 2011, 2013), with effects lasting up to 3 hours .

An interesting result made possible by the use of binocular rivalry (and more generally, bistable stimulation; i.e. Maruya, Yang, & Blake, 2007) is that it is susceptible to influence from cross-modal signals. For instance, it was demonstrated that an appropriate haptic stimulation is able to promote visual dominance of the congruent rivaling stimulus during binocular rivalry, as well as simultaneously reducing its suppression (Lunghi, Binda, & Morrone, 2010).

A couple of compelling questions are, what is the temporal dynamic of this cross-moral interaction with binocular rivalry? Does the ocular

(14)

imbalance caused by the monocular deprivation selectively interact with the cross-modal cues?

The motivation for the first part of this work, therefore, is to try to combine these proofs and techniques: we propose an experiment in which the ocular balance of the participants is heavily disrupted in favor of the deprived eye after 2 hours and a half of monocular deprivation. We then engage them in binocular rivalry while they perform haptic exploration of a grating that could only be congruent with one of the two rivaling visual gratings separately presented to the two eyes.

0.2 CROSS-MODAL INTERACTIONS OUTSIDE OF AWARENESS

Cross-modal interactions – when a signal from one modality is able to interact with and modulate a signal coming from another modality – between vision and other modalities are possible outside of observers’ visual awareness. A striking example is observed when the interaction occurs between a visual stimulus that is being actively suppressed and a signal from another modality (Deroy et al., 2016). Binocular rivalry and its variants, for instance, represent unique tools as they offer convenient

(15)

ways to promote inter-ocular competition and the consequent active suppression of one of the two images presented to the eyes.

As introduced in the previous paragraph, a number of recent works have shown that haptic signals presented during binocular rivalry can promote the dominance of the congruent visual stimulus as well as reducing its suppression (Lunghi, Binda, & Morrone, 2010; Lunghi & Alais, 2013, 2015). Binocular rivalry, however, does not unambiguously allow to determine whether this effect is a result of the boosting of the other modality’s signal on the congruent visual stimulus, or if on the contrary it ensues from a suppression on the incongruent, competing visual stimulus. A better tool to answer to this question is a variant of binocular rivalry called continuous flash suppression (CFS). This technique was originally developed and described by Tsuchiya and Koch in 2005: the image presented to one eye is suppressed by the continuous presentation of high-contrast geometric patterns known as “Mondrians” at rates of around 10 Hz.

This suppression is stronger, deeper and more reliably achieved and maintained compared to canonical binocular rivalry between two static images (Tsuchiya, Koch, Gilroy, & Blake, 2006), allowing for a far tighter control over the suppression of the visual signal. Since only one static

(16)

image is rendered invisible by the constantly changing Mondrian patterns presented to the other eye, CFS allows to unambiguously ascribe possible cross-modal interaction to one or the other explanation, unlike traditional binocular rivalry.

In the second part of this work, therefore, we present experiments that directly test whether cross-modal interactions can rescue a congruent but deeply suppressed visual stimulus while it is invisible to the observer.

0.3 RECALIBRATING TIME PERCEPTION TO SELF-MOTION

Perception of duration is influenced by several factors and variables: for instance, eye-movements and voluntary actions alter the subjective perception of time (Morrone et al., 2005; Tomassini & Morrone, 2016). In the case of moving stimuli, temporal durations seem to dilate and to last for a longer time as opposed to stationary stimuli of the same physical duration (Kanai et al., 2006).

Therefore, it seems reasonable to investigate the link between time perception and motion perception, as even today there is much discussion about the nature of the neural machinery required to perceive time. A long-standing debate is whether time perception arises from a centralized

(17)

clock (Treisman, Faulkner, Naish, & Brogan, 1990) or whether is a result of localized, task- and modality-dependent structures distributed across the brain. Growing evidence has accumulated around the latter hypothesis, with, perhaps, the strongest point being that adapting to motion causes a reduced duration perception for stimuli presented only in that portion of space and not in other, non-adapted regions (Johnston, Arnold, & Nishida, 2006). This spatial selectivity, moreover, is for position in the external space, rather than on retinal space (Burr, Cicchini, Arrighi, & Morrone, 2011).

An interesting investigation would involve independent and reliable manipulations of both true stimulus motion as well as probing the effect of self-motion in a stationary world. However, traditional visual psychophysics, usually involving stimuli presentations on desktop displays and fixed, stationary set-ups, renders this independent selective manipulation impractical and difficult to properly achieve.

The third work we propose in this thesis was explicitly designed to independently gauge the contributions of stimulus- and self-motion on duration perception. We used virtual reality to create an immersive and believable virtual environment in which observers could walk, while also finely recording their movements. This allowed us to dissociate

(18)

self-motion from stimulus self-motion in a way that is not easily achievable with more traditional set-ups.

0.4 STRUCTURE OF THE THESIS

In this work, we present three studies in which we employed different techniques and approaches in an attempt to investigate the ability of our nervous system to cope with sudden changes in our sensory landscape and to adapt to it in order to maximize the quality of the information obtained by our sensory systems. The research abstracts produced for each of the three presented works follow.

(19)

0.4.1 Abstracts

0.4.1.1 Early Cross-modal Plasticity in Adults1

It is known that, after a prolonged period of visual deprivation, the adult visual cortex can be recruited for nonvisual processing, reflecting cross-modal plasticity. Here, we investigated whether cross-modal plasticity can occur at short timescales in the typical adult brain by comparing the interaction between vision and touch during binocular rivalry before and after a brief period of monocular deprivation, which strongly alters ocular balance favoring the deprived eye. While viewing dichoptically two gratings of orthogonal orientation, participants were asked to actively explore a haptic grating congruent in orientation to one of the two rivalrous stimuli. We repeated this procedure before and after 150 min of monocular deprivation. We first confirmed that haptic stimulation interacted with vision during rivalry promoting dominance of the congruent visuo-haptic stimulus and that monocular deprivation increased the deprived eye and decreased the nondeprived eye dominance. Interestingly, after deprivation, we found that the effect of touch did not change for the nondeprived eye, whereas it disappeared for the deprived eye, which was potentiated after deprivation. The absence of

(20)

visuo-haptic interaction for the deprived eye lasted for over 1 hr and was not attributable to a masking induced by the stronger response of the deprived eye as confirmed by a control experiment. Taken together, our results demonstrate that the adult human visual cortex retains a high degree of cross-modal plasticity, which can occur even at very short timescales.

0.4.1.2 Touch Accelerates Visual Awareness2

To efficiently interact with the external environment, our nervous system combines information arising from different sensory modalities. Recent evidence suggests that cross-modal interactions can be automatic and even unconscious, reflecting the ecological relevance of cross-modal processing. Here, we use continuous flash suppression (CFS) to directly investigate whether haptic signals can interact with visual signals outside of visual awareness. We measured suppression durations of visual gratings rendered invisible by CFS either during visual stimulation alone or during visuo-haptic stimulation. We found that active exploration of a haptic grating congruent in orientation with the suppressed visual grating reduced suppression durations both compared with visual-only

(21)

stimulation and to incongruent visuo-haptic stimulation. We also found that the facilitatory effect of touch on visual suppression disappeared when the visual and haptic gratings were mismatched in either spatial frequency or orientation. Together, these results demonstrate that congruent touch can accelerate the rise to consciousness of a suppressed visual stimulus and that this unconscious cross-modal interaction depends on visuo-haptic congruency. Furthermore, since CFS suppression is thought to occur early in visual cortical processing, our data reinforce the evidence suggesting that visuo-haptic interactions can occur at the earliest stages of cortical processing.

0.4.1.3 Apparent motion causes the time dilation effect3

It is known that moving visual stimuli are perceived to last longer than stationary stimuli with the same physical duration (Kanai, Paffen, Hogendoorn, & Verstraten, 2006), and that motor actions (Tomassini & Morrone, 2016) and eye movements (Morrone, Ross, & Burr, 2005) can alter perceived durations. In the present work, we investigated the contributions of stimulus motion and self-motion to perceived duration while observers stood or walked in a virtual reality environment. Using a

(22)

visual temporal reproduction task, we independently manipulated both the participants’ motion (stationary or walking) and the stimulus motion (retinal stationary, real-world stationary and negative double velocity). When the observers were standing still, drifting gratings were perceived as lasting longer than duration-matched static gratings. Interestingly, we did not see any time distortion when observers were walking, neither when the gratings were kept stationary relative to the observer’s point of view (i.e., no retinal motion) nor when they were stationary in the external world (i.e., producing the same retinal velocity as the walking condition with stationary grating). Self-motion only produced a significant duration dilation when the gratings were moving at double speed, opposite to the observers’ walking direction. Consistent with previous work (Fornaciai, Arrighi, & Burr, 2016), this suggests that the system is able to suppress self-generated motion in order to enhance external motion, which would have ecological benefits, for example, for threat detection while navigating through the environment.

1 Lo Verde, L., Morrone, M. C., & Lunghi, C. (2017). Early Cross-modal Plasticity in Adults.

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 29(3), 520–529. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01067

2 Lunghi, C., Lo Verde, L., & Alais, D. (2017). Touch Accelerates Visual Awareness. I-Perception,

8(1), 204166951668698. https://doi.org/10.1177/2041669516686986

3 Lo Verde, L., Alais, D., Burr, D. C., Morrone, M. C., MacDougall, H., & Verstraten, F. (2018).

The time dilation effect in an active observer and virtual environment requires apparent motion: no dilation for retinal- or world-motion alone, under review.

(23)
(24)

1 Early Cross-modal Plasticity in Adults

1.1 INTRODUCTION

Neuroplasticity, the intrinsic capability of the nervous system to change and adapt as a function of physiologic changes, sensory experiences, and environmental pressures (Pascual-Leone, Amedi, Fregni, & Merabet, 2005), is maximal early in life, during the so-called critical period (Berardi, Pizzorusso, & Maffei, 2000; Hubel & Wiesel, 1970). During the critical period, neuroplasticity is so high that, in case of sensory loss, neural circuits can be dramatically reorganized (Merabet & Pascual-Leone, 2010; Pascual-Leone et al., 2005). For example, the primary visual cortex of early blind participants is recruited for tactile (Amedi, Raz, Azulay, Malach, & Zohary, 2010; Sathian, 2005; Hamilton & Pascual-Leone, 1998; Sadato et al., 1996) and auditory (Collignon et al., 2011; Gougoux, Zatorre, Lassonde, Voss, & Lepore, 2005) sensory processing, a phenomenon usually referred as cross-modal plasticity. Although neuroplasticity in higher-level association brain regions, like those important for learning and memory, remains high in adults (Fuchs & Flugge, 2014), it is very limited in low-level sensory cortices in adult brains (Berardi et al., 2000). However, some crossmodal plasticity is retained. For example, the primary

(25)

visual cortex can be transiently recruited for tactile processing in normally sighted individuals after prolonged (5 days) blindfolding (Merabet et al., 2008; Kauffman, Theoret, & Pascual-Leone, 2002). This suggests that visual deprivation unmasks somatosensory signals in V1 rather than being a rewiring through the formation of new pathways (for this debate, see Qin & Yu, 2013; Striem-Amit, Cohen, Dehaene, & Amedi, 2012). In line with this hypothesis, primary visual cortical activity (BOLD) in the absence of visual stimulation has been reported during different haptic exploration tasks (Snow, Strother, & Humphreys, 2014; Merabet et al., 2007; Saito, Okada, Honda, Yonekura, & Sadato, 2006). Consistent with the suggestion of the unmasking hypothesis, a recent study by Convento, Vallar, Galantini, and Bolognini (2013) has shown increased visual cortical excitability (measured as decreased TMS phosphene thresholds) during tactile and auditory stimulation (Convento et al., 2013). One efficient way of studying early cross-modal interactions is using ambiguous visual stimuli and, in particular, binocular rivalry, a specific form of perceptual bistability that takes place when two incompatible images are separately delivered to each eye (Alais & Blake, 2005; Blake & Logothetis, 2002; Levelt, 1965). During binocular rivalry, instead of merging the two monocular images, the observer perceives a periodic alternation of the

(26)

two visual stimuli that compete against each other to gain access to the observer’s visual awareness. Whereas one image dominates observer’s perception, the other, albeit displayed on the retina, is suppressed from visual awareness. Binocular rivalry suppression occurs early in the visual system: Neural activity related to the suppressed visual stimulus is not detectable outside V1 or V2 (for a review, see Sterzer, Stein, Ludwig, Rothkirch, & Hesselmann, 2014). The existence of cross-modal connections to early visual areas is also supported by the findings that crossmodal signals can interact with the suppressed visual stimulus (for a review, see Deroy et al., 2016) during binocular rivalry or with the suppressed information during continuous flash suppression (Tsuchiya & Koch, 2005): Auditory (Lunghi, Morrone, & Alais, 2014; Alsius & Munhall, 2013; Conrad et al., 2013; Conrad, Bartels, Kleiner, & Noppeney, 2010), tactile (Lunghi & Alais, 2013, 2015; Lunghi & Morrone, 2013; Lunghi, Binda, & Morrone, 2010), combined auditory–tactile (Lunghi et al., 2014), olfactory (Zhou, Jiang, He, & Chen, 2010), proprioceptive (Salomon, Lim, Herbelin, Hesselmann, & Blanke, 2013), and vestibular (Salomon, Kaliuzhna, Herbelin, & Blanke, 2015) signals have been shown to interact with the suppressed visual signal during binocular rivalry, reinforcing the animal data showing direct cross-modal input in V1 (Clavagnier, Falchier,

(27)

& Kennedy, 2004; Rockland & Ojima, 2003; Falchier, Clavagnier, Barone, & Kennedy, 2002). In particular, Lunghi et al. (2010) showed that actively exploring a haptic grating congruent in orientation with one of two rivalrous visual gratings could disambiguate visual perception during binocular rivalry, both prolonging dominance and shortening suppression of the visual grating congruent in orientation with the haptic one. To observe the effect, the haptic grating needed to be matched in spatial frequency (Lunghi et al., 2010), orientation (Lunghi & Alais, 2013), and spatial position (Lunghi & Morrone, 2013) to the visual stimulus. Taken together, these results support the idea that somatosensory signals can indeed reach early visual cortices, probably V1, given the high orientation and spatial frequency selectivity of the interaction that matches closely the selectivity of the primary visual cortex neurons. Binocular rivalry is a robust technique also to measure small plastic changes in ocular balance: 150 min of monocular deprivation considerably alter the dynamics of binocular rivalry, counterintuitively boosting the deprived eye signal (Lunghi, Burr, & Morrone, 2011). After deprivation, the deprived eye strongly dominates visual perception: The mean phase duration of the deprived eye becomes twice as long as that of the non-deprived eye, reflecting homeostatic plasticity. This effect decays over time, lasting up to

(28)

3 hr (Lunghi, Burr, & Morrone, 2013; Lunghi et al., 2011), and suggests that the adult visual cortex retains a high degree of experiencedependent plasticity (Lunghi, Berchicci, Morrone, & Di Russo, 2015; Lunghi et al., 2011, 2013). Here, we investigated whether cross-modal plasticity can occur also at short timescales in the adult visual cortex of normally sighted humans. We combined the visuo-haptic cross-modal paradigm described in Lunghi et al. (2010) with a short period of monocular deprivation. The data show that monocular deprivation interferes with the haptic modulation of binocular rivalry, but only for the deprived eye.

(29)

1.2 METHODS

1.2.1 Participants

Eight participants (five women; mean age = 28.4 years, SD = 2.9 years), including two authors of this study, participated in the main experiment. Five of these participants also participated in the control experiment. All of the participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, no strong eye preference (measured as eye predominance in binocular rivalry), and normal stereo acuity (Frisby Stereotest; Sasieni, 1978). Except for the two authors, all participants were naive to the purposes of the experiment.

1.2.2 Ethic Statement

The experimental protocol was approved by the Tuscany Regional Ethics Committee of the Azienda Ospedaliero-Universitaria Meyer and was performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All of the participants gave written informed consent.

1.2.3 Apparatus and Stimuli

The experiment took place in a quiet room in total darkness to remove any spatial frame of reference. The visual stimuli, developed in

(30)

MATLAB (Version 7.11.0; The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA) using Psychtoolbox-3 (Kleiner et al., 2007; Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997), were two superimposed oblique orthogonal red and blue gratings (orientation = ±45°, size = 3°, SF = 2 c/deg, 50% of the maximum contrast) surrounded by a white smoothed circle, presented on a black uniform background (luminance = 0.17 cd/m2 ) in central vision. The peak luminance of the red

grating was matched with the peak luminance of the blue one (1.7 cd/m2).

Dichoptic stimulation was achieved by having participants wear red and blue anaglyph goggles. The gratings contained a small (0.2°) white fixation dot in their center and were presented on a 24-in. LCD display (Acer LCD GD245HQ) positioned horizontally at 37 cm above a horizontal mirror (see Figure 1.1A). Observers viewed the stimuli reflected in the mirror at a distance of 35 cm from the observers’ eyes. Placed 35 cm under the mirror, the haptic stimulus consisted of a 3-D-printed sinusoidal grating (diameter = 3 cm, 2 cycles/cm of spatial frequency). The grating was mounted on a servomotor, which could rotate it arbitrarily at any given orientation. The visual and haptic stimuli were spatially aligned so that observers experienced the illusion of a single visual and haptic object. The onset and offset of a touch period were measured monitoring an LED and a photoresistor positioned laterally to the 3D grating (see Figure 1.1B).

(31)

Figure 1.1. Experimental setup and paradigm. (A) Diagram illustrating the

experimental setup: The rivalrous gratings were presented on an LCD monitor and reflected by an opaque mirror onto the haptic stimulus (grooved grating) location. (B) The haptic grating orientation was varied at each touch period by a servomotor. The temporal dynamics of haptic exploration were finely tracked with an LED-photoresistor system. (C) Experimental paradigm: After baseline measurements, the observers wore a translucent eye patch on their dominant eye for 150 min. After the deprivation, 8 × 4 min measurements were collected at 0, 5, 10, 15, 30, 45, 60, and 90 min from the patch removal.

When the observer’s finger touched the grating patch, it occluded the LED light to the photoresistor, signaling the onset of touch. Vice versa, when the observer lifted the finger at the end of a touch period, the light from the LED activated the photoresistor again, signaling the offset of touch. The servomotor, LED, and photoresistor were controlled by an

(32)

Arduino UNO R3 microcontroller (D’Ausilio, 2012) interfaced to MATLAB using the Arduino IO Package (www.mathworks.com/ matlabcentral/fileexchange/32374-legacy-matlab-andsimulink-support-for-arduino). Short-term monocular deprivation was achieved by having observers wear a translucent eye patch on their dominant eye, defined as the eye that perceptually dominated for most of the time during preliminary binocular rivalry measurements for each participant. The eye patch was made of translucent plastic material and blocked all patterned visual contrast without causing dark adaptation (attenuation = 15%; for details, see Lunghi et al., 2011).

1.2.4 Task and Procedure

1.2.4.1 Main Experiment

During each 240-s experimental block, participants continuously reported which visual grating they were consciously perceiving: red, blue, or mixed. Observers reported the rivalrous perception by continuous alternate mouse button press with their non-dominant hand. The association between grating color (red or blue) and orientation (+45° or −45° in relation to vertical orientation) was swapped after each trial to avoid adaptation and counterbalance for eye dominance. At randomized

(33)

time intervals (time between two consecutive touch periods = 8.54 ± 0.49 s), the fixation point shape changed from circular to squared, signaling to the observer the beginning of a touch period. Observers were instructed to actively explore the haptic grating by performing horizontal movements using their dominant hand’s index finger while reporting their visual perception by pressing the appropriate mouse button with the non-dominant hand. The duration of each touch period was randomized, lasting 2.63 ± 0.34 s on average. After at least four training experimental blocks, each observer performed 2 × 240 sec measurements before the beginning of deprivation, which served as baseline, and 8 × 240 s measurements acquired at 0, 5, 10, 15, 30, 45, 60, and 90 min after the eye patch removal (see Figure 1.1C). This procedure (2 × baseline measurements + 150-min monocular deprivation + 8 × after deprivation measurements) was repeated independently four times in different days for each participant.

1.2.4.2 Control Experiment

The visual stimuli contrast was varied to simulate the effect of monocular deprivation (simulated deprived eye contrast: mean = 0.77, SD = 0.1; non-deprived eye: mean = 0.22, SD = 0.1). For each participant, the

(34)

appropriate contrasts were determined by increasing the contrast on the eye that was deprived in the previous session and decreasing the contrast of the non-deprived eye to achieve a difference in the perceptual dominance comparable with that induced by monocular deprivation. We collected 4 × 240 s experimental blocks for each participant following the same experimental procedure of the main experiment.

1.2.5 Data Analyses

Before the deprivation, baseline measurements were acquired for each participant. Data from 240-s blocks after deprivation were averaged in the intervals of 0–19, 30–49, and 60–94 min after eye patch removal. We first computed for each touch and no-touch stimulation period the probability of maintaining the same visual percept for the whole period, switching perception once or switching more than once, conditioned to the type of visuo-haptic stimulation (congruent, incongruent orientation, no touch), separately for the two eyes (deprived and non-deprived). The touch periods that started during a period of mixed rivalry, which cannot be used to compute the visuo-haptic congruence given that both visual orientations were perceived simultaneously, were very rare (mixed rivalry:

(35)

mean = 2.4%, SD = 1.3%). We also computed the time course of the effect of haptic exploration on the dynamics of binocular rivalry by calculating the probability of seeing the visual stimulus congruent with the haptic one as a function of time elapsed from the onset of haptic stimulation. Each participant’s tracking of perceptual alternations for the 11 s after the onset of each touch period was overlaid and averaged every 440 ms. Separate probability traces were obtained for the deprived and non-deprived eyes. To obtain a visual dominance index for each eye independently from the effect of touch, we averaged for each participant the last six bins (2.64 s) of each probability trace. Calculating the index using the last 5 s of the traces produced very similar results. Finally, we also calculated a touch effect index for each eye by computing the integral of the area of the probability trace from touch onset to the time in which the probability decayed to visual-only levels.

1.2.5.1 Statistics

To compare the effect of touch on binocular rivalry, we used paired-sample, two-tailed Student’s t tests (congruent, incongruent, and visual only). Each probability trace time bin was compared across eyes using paired-sample t tests. For the comparison between the areas of the

(36)

probability curves during the haptic exploration (touch effect indexes) and the respective vision-only predominance indexes, we used bootstrap sign tests with 10000 repetitions, and for the comparison between the touch effect indexes and the null touch effect, we used one-sample, two-tailed Student’s t tests.

1.3 RESULTS

1.3.1 Main Experiment

We measured the effect of haptic stimulation on the dynamics of binocular rivalry before and after 150 min of monocular deprivation (a schematic representation of the experimental setup and paradigm is shown in Figure 1.1). The probability of maintaining or switching perception during a touch period conditioned to the type of visuo-haptic stimulation (parallel, orthogonal, or visual only) was calculated separately for the two eyes. Consistent with Lunghi et al. (2010), before deprivation (Figure 1.2), the probability of maintaining was significantly higher for congruent visuo-haptic stimulation compared with vision-only stimulation (Figure 1.2A, deprived eye: paired sample, two-tailed t test, t(7) = 4.91, Bonferroni–Holms corrected α = .0167, p = .0017; Figure 1.2B,

(37)

non-deprived eye: paired-sample, two-tailed t test, Bonferroni–Holms corrected α = .0167, t(7) = 6.95, p = .0002).

On the other hand, the probability of switching perception was significantly higher for incongruent visuo-haptic stimulation compared with vision-only stimulation for the deprived eye (Figure 1.2A, deprived eye: paired-sample, two-tailed t test, Bonferroni–Holms corrected α = .025, t(7) = 3.49, p = .01).

(38)

Figure 1.2. Average probabilities across touch conditions, before monocular

deprivation (baseline) separately for the two eyes. The average probabilities of maintaining the same visual percept (orange bar), switching visual perception once (green bar), or switching more than once during a touch period (gray bar), conditioned to the type of visuo-haptic stimulation (congruent, incongruent orientation, or no touch)—(A) deprived eye and (B) non-deprived eye—error bars represent 1 ± SEM. The probability of maintaining the same visual percept for the whole touch period was significantly higher for the congruent visuo-haptic stimulation for both eyes compared with the no-touch stimulation, whereas the probability of switching visual percept was significantly higher for the incongruent (orthogonal) visuo-haptic stimulation for both eyes compared with the no-touch stimulation (paired-sample, two-tailed t test: n = 8, *p ≤ .05, **p ≤ .01, ***p ≤ .001, ‡p ≤ .05; however, the test did not survive the correction for multiple comparisons).

(39)

Figure 1.3. Dynamics of the effect of touch on binocular rivalry. Proportion of

reported dominance of the visual stimulus congruent in orientation with the haptic orientation plotted as a function of time from the touch onset for the deprived (red symbols) and non-deprived (blue symbols) eyes. These curves were obtained by overlaying and averaging each participant’s perceptual tracking for 11 sec after each touch onset. (A) Before deprivation. The probability of seeing the visual orientation congruent with the haptic orientation increases and slowly reverts to baseline levels for both eyes after touch offset. (B) At 0–14 min after the end of deprivation. (C) At 30–49 min after the end of deprivation. (D) At 60–94 min after the end of deprivation. After deprivation, the effect of touch is absent for the deprived eye. Horizontal red and blue lines are the average proportion of dominance during visual-only stimulation for the deprived and non-deprived eyes. Vertical dashed line shows the average of the touch interval. Bin width of 0.44 sec. Asterisks represent statistical significance: deprived eye versus baseline (red asterisks), non-deprived eye versus baseline (blue asterisks), and deprived eye versus non-deprived eye (black asterisks; *p ≤ .05, **p ≤ .01, ***p ≤ .001). Error bars represent 1 ± SEM.

(40)

For the non-deprived eye, the effect did not survive the correction for multiple comparisons (Figure 1.2B, non-deprived eye: paired-sample, two-tailed t test, t(7) = 3.01, Bonferroni–Holms corrected α = 0.0167, p = .02), although there was a clear trend. Both sets of data indicate that haptic stimulation interacted with binocular rivalry both by prolonging dominance and by shortening suppression of the congruent visual stimulus. We then analyzed the dynamics of the visuo-haptic effect during binocular rivalry. Figure 1.3 (red symbols = deprived eye, blue symbols = non-deprived eye) shows the data compared with the average proportion of dominance of either the deprived (red dashed line) or non-deprived (blue dashed line) eye measured during the last 2.64 s of visual-only stimulation. Before monocular deprivation (Figure 1.3A), haptic stimulation increased the probability to perceive the congruent visual grating for both the deprived and non-deprived eyes during the final phase of the touch period. In line with previous work (Lunghi et al., 2011, 2013), monocular deprivation increased the deprived eye dominance and decreased the non-deprived eye dominance. Eye dominance (mean ± SEM) before deprivation (Figure 1.3A) was 50 ± 3% for the deprived eye and 50 ± 2% for the non-deprived eye. During the first 19 min after eye patch removal (Figure 1.3B), the deprived eye dominance significantly

(41)

increased compared with baseline measurements (+10%, paired-sample, two-tailed t test, Bonferroni–Holms corrected α = .0167, t(7) = 4.68, p = .002), whereas the non-deprived eye dominance significantly decreased (−10%, paired-sample, two-tailed t test, t(7) = −3.37, Bonferroni–Holms corrected α = .025, p = .012). The effect of deprivation was still significant 49 min after eye patch removal for the non-deprived eye (−7%, paired-sample, two-tailed t test, t(7) = −3.62, Bonferroni–Holms corrected α = .0167, p = .009), whereas it did not survive the correction for multiple comparisons for the deprived eye (+5%, paired-sample t test, t(7) = 2.52, Bonferroni–Holms corrected α = .025, p = .04). The effect was not significant for both eyes 94 min after deprivation, although a clear trend was present (+5% for the deprived eye, −4% for the non-deprived eye). Together with the baseline effect, there was a differential effect of the touch for the deprived and non-deprived eyes. The probability of perceiving the congruent touch orientation with respect to the baseline significantly increased for the non-deprived eye as for the data before deprivation. However, we never observed any touch effect for the deprived eye, even after 94 min after the deprivation. At this time, the ocular balance is not statistically different from the one obtained before deprivation. Nevertheless, touch is not able to induce an increase in

(42)

dominance of the congruent percept (Figure 1.3B–D). To quantify the effect of touch at different times before and after deprivation, we obtained an index computing the integral of the smoothed probability curves from touch onset to the decay time to baseline value (colored areas in Figure 1.4 showing, for visualization purposes only, the average probability traces after Gaussian temporal smoothing).

(43)

Figure 1.4. Average effect of touch. The average effect of touch on binocular

rivalry is represented by the area subtended by the curve between touch onset and the time that the baseline values are reached. The left column (A, C, E, and G, in warm colors) refers to the deprived eye, whereas the right column (B, D, F, and H, in cold

(44)

colors) refers to the non-deprived eye. The lighter colors represents the effect of touch extending after touch offset (dashed vertical line) until the curve decayed to baseline (dotted vertical line). The darker colors represent negative areas, that is, portions of the curve that lied below the vision-only base proportion. The probability curves are the same of those in Figure 1.3, after being smoothed with a Gaussian function with constant equal to 2.4 s — this was done only for visualization purposes.

Individual observers’ data are reported in Figure 1.5. Before deprivation (Figure 1.5A), the effect of touch was comparable for the deprived and non-deprived eyes (deprived eye touch effect index: 0.31 ± 0.11, non-deprived eye: 0.32 ± 0.1; two-tailed bootstrap sign test: n = 10000, α = .05, p = .98, ns). During the first 19 min after eye patch removal (Figure 1.5B), the effect of touch was significantly stronger for the non-deprived eye compared with the non-deprived eye (non-deprived eye: 0.01 ± 0.13, non-deprived eye: 0.42 ± 0.13; bootstrap sign test: n = 10000, α = .05, p = .001). A clear trend of a stronger effect of touch for the non-deprived eye was present up to 49 min after stimulus onset (Figure 1.5C; deprived eye: 0.17 ± 0.12, non-deprived eye: 0.41 ± 0.17; bootstrap sign test: n = 10000, α = .05, p = .064), decaying to pre-deprivation values 90 min after deprivation (Figure 1.5D; deprived eye: 0.19 ± 0.16, non-deprived eye: 0.39 ± 0.17; two-tailed bootstrap sign test: n = 10000, α = .05, p = .14, ns).

(45)

The data for the deprived eye after deprivation are scattered around zero (Figure 1.5B–D), indicating no effect of touch (one-sample, two-tailed t test, H0: X = 0, all ps > .07), whereas for the non-deprived eye, the

individual participants’ data were significantly different from zero (one-sample, two-tailed t test, H0: X = 0, all ps < .009).

(46)

Figure 1.5. Individual participants’ data. The touch index (computed for each

participant as the area subtended by the probability curves as illustrated in Figure 1.4) on the deprived eye plotted for each participant against the touch index for the non-deprived eye measured before (A) and at different times after deprivation (B–D). The black star represents the average across all participants, and the error bars represent 1 ± SEM. After deprivation (B, C, and D), the magnitude of the visuo-haptic effect is higher for the non-deprived eye than the deprived eye (data points scattering above the equality dashed black line). The vertical and horizontal gray dashed lines represent the absence of visuo-haptic effect.

(47)

1.3.2 Deprivation Control Experiment

After deprivation, the baseline values of the two eyes are very different, with the deprived eye approaching 60% of predominance. This may generate a flooring effect that does not allow to measure reliably the influence of touch on the deprived eye. The stronger predominance of the deprived eye might also mask the presence of the haptic modulation. To investigate whether the absence of touch effect that we found for the deprived eye could be attributable to the different strength of the visual response, we performed a control experiment using stimuli of different contrast in the two eyes. The contrast difference was set to simulate the effect of deprivation in the unbalance of the phase duration (mean simulated deprived eye contrast: 0.78 ± 0.1, mean simulated non-deprived eye contrast: 0.22 ± 0.1).

(48)

Figure 1.6. (A) Individual participants’ data for the control experiment. A is the

same as Figure 1.5, but for the control experiment. B is the same as Figure 1.3, but for the control experiment. Error bars represent 1 ± SEM.

Figure 1.6A reports the individual participants’ data for the effect of touch on the simulated deprived (high-contrast stimulus) and non-deprived eye (low contrast stimulus), whereas Figure 1.6B reports the probability traces for perceiving the touch congruent percept. Despite the factor of 3.67 difference in contrast and the factor of 1.38 difference in baseline eye dominance, the overall effect of touch is equal for the two eyes (simulated deprived eye = 0.29 ± 0.13, simulated non-deprived eye = 0.29 ± 0.07; two-tailed bootstrap sign test: n = 10000, α = .05, p = .99, ns). These indexes were also statistically different from zero (which indicates no effect of touch) for both eyes (one-sample, two-tailed t tests, simulated deprived eye: t(4) = 4.24, p = .013; simulated non-deprived eye: t(4) =

(49)

7.99, p = .0013). In addition, the temporal dynamics of the effect is very similar (Figure 1.6B) with both curves returning at baseline at the same time from touch onset.

1.4 DISCUSSION

We have demonstrated a new form of rapid cross-modal plasticity in adult normally sighted humans by showing that short-term monocular deprivation alters the interaction between visual and haptic signals during binocular rivalry. After deprivation, the effect of touch on the potentiated deprived eye disappears, whereas it remains unchanged for the non-deprived eye. This differential effect of touch for the two eyes lasts for over 1 hr after eye patch removal and is not reducible to a change in visual signal strength. As reported by previous studies (Lunghi et al., 2011, 2013), short-term monocular deprivation counterintuitively boosts the deprived eye’s signal, prolonging dominance durations of the deprived eye, while shortening those of the non-deprived eye during binocular rivalry (Lunghi et al., 2011, 2013). This effect could be mediated by a homeostatic upregulation of gain control mechanisms in the attempt to compensate for the lack of visual input of the deprived eye. This is coherent with the

(50)

observed increase in the apparent contrast for the images delivered to the deprived eye, which can appear as much as 36% higher compared with the non-deprived eye (Lunghi et al., 2011), pointing to fast, cortical gain adjustment mechanisms. Interestingly, the homeostatic plasticity induced by short-term monocular deprivation has been shown to arise in the primary visual cortex, as 150 min of monocular deprivation alter the earliest component of the visual evoked potential (Lunghi, Berchicci, et al., 2015) and the GABA resting level at the calcarine cortex (Lunghi, Emir, Morrone, & Bridge, 2015). This suggests that the differential visuo-haptic interactions that we found after deprivation arise early in the visual system. The suppression mechanisms acting during rivalry between two orthogonal orientations are thought to occur early in the visual processing hierarchy (reviewed in Sterzer et al., 2014). We found that the haptic signal is able to interact with the suppressed visual signal during binocular rivalry, again suggesting an early locus of signal interaction. The measured differences in the haptic effect between the deprived and non-deprived eyes cannot be explained as a result of a mere deprivation-induced change in low-level stimulus strength but are likely caused by a plastic change of the processing of the visual information induced by the deprivation. The control experiment showed that haptic stimulation

(51)

affected equally low- and high-contrasted rivalrous stimuli, reinforcing the suggestion that the selective lack of cross-modal interactions observed after deprivation originates from a rapid plastic change of cross-modal processing, because changing the visual signal strength by varying the visual stimuli contrast did not alter the cross-modal interaction for both the deprived and non-deprived eyes. Our results are consistent with a large literature of cross-modal plasticity: The ability of the nervous system to enhance the most appropriate sensory modalities/aspects given the past experiences is explained as an attempt to use the best information the system has reliable access to. For example, the occipital recruitment in blind people for tactile processing (Collignon et al., 2011; Amedi et al., 2010; Gougoux et al., 2005; Sathian, 2005; Hamilton & Pascual-Leone, 1998; Sadato et al., 1996) is explained as the request of the system to dedicate optimal processing to a modality that became critical for the exploration of the environment. Consistent with this hypothesis, many haptic performances in the blind people are better than those in sighted people (for a review, see Merabet & Pascual-Leone, 2010). Similar effects can be induced also in sighted people after training combined with prolonged blindfolding (Merabet et al., 2008; Kauffman et al., 2002). After 5 days of binocular deprivation, the primary visual cortex of normally

(52)

sighted individuals is recruited for tactile processing (Merabet et al., 2008; Kauffman et al., 2002). These studies suggest that the occipital recruitment by other modalities can result from a rapid and adaptive rearrangement of the neural weights triggered by a strong enough perturbation of the sensory experience stream. They also point out that the haptic signals are probably already present in the occipital cortex, but their efficiency is low in normally sighted people. Long-term visual deprivation can reinforce these cross-modal signals in response to the decreased visual signal saliency. Here, we showed that cross-modal plasticity could occur also in the opposite direction and at short timescales: After short-term monocular deprivation, the deprived eye signal gain is homeostatically upregulated, and the haptic signal, which normally interacts with the visual one, is reduced. Whether this reduction is due to a change in the haptic signal strength or a change in the cross-modal combination mechanism is, at present, unknown. However, on the basis of the control experiment, we can dismiss the masking hypothesis, according to which the stronger deprived eye signal masks the haptic one without involving a change at the cross-modal level. However, what are the possible mechanisms that induce this rapid rearrangement of cross-modal interactions? van Loon et al. (2013) recently showed that the

(53)

dynamics of binocular rivalry are correlated to GABA concentrations in the primary visual cortex as measured by magnetic resonance spectroscopy, supporting the idea that bistable visual perception critically depends on excitatory/inhibitory balance in the visual cortex (van Loon et al., 2013; Alais, 2012; Tong, Meng, & Blake, 2006). By using the same technique, Lunghi et al. (2015) showed that a brief period of monocular deprivation induces a decrease of intracortical GABA concentration in the primary visual cortex and that the decrease strongly correlates with the perceptual ocular dominance changes caused by the monocular deprivation on binocular rivalry (Lunghi, Emir, et al., 2015). It is therefore possible that the short-term cross-modal plasticity that we observed here is also mediated by a transient decrease of intracortical inhibition induced by monocular deprivation. Studies on animal models also suggest that homeostatic plasticity is triggered by changes in the excitation/inhibition balance in the primary visual cortex (Maffei & Turrigiano, 2008), either mediated by a downregulation of GABAergic synapses (Maffei, Nelson, & Turrigiano, 2004) or an upregulation of excitatory neurons (Wang, Fontanini, & Maffei, 2012), and further animal and magnetic resonance spectroscopy work is needed to elucidate the exact molecular mechanisms. In conclusion, the plastic and adaptive readjustment of the

(54)

early visuo-haptic interactions demonstrated here strongly suggests that the adult nervous system retains a high degree of cross-modal plasticity that operates also at short timescales.

(55)
(56)

2 Touch Accelerates Visual Awareness

2.1 INTRODUCTION

Combining information arising from different sensory modalities is essential to interact efficiently with the environment (Alais, Newell, & Mamassian, 2010). While multisensory integration—the merging the information from different modalities in a single percept —is not mandatory (Ernst & Banks, 2002; Hillis, Ernst, Banks, & Landy, 2002) and relies on attention (van Ee, van Boxtel, Parker, & Alais, 2009) and awareness (for a review on multimodal awareness, see Deroy, Chen, & Spence, 2014), cross-modal interactions might be more automatic. Cross-modal interactions occur when a signal from one sensory Cross-modality is modulated by a signal from another modality. Growing evidence suggests that cross-modal interactions between vision and other modalities can occur outside of visual awareness, such as when a visual stimulus presented to one eye undergoes a temporary suppression due to a conflicting image presented to the other eye (reviewed in Deroy et al., 2016; Faivre, Salomon, & Blanke, 2015). Studying such ‘‘unconscious’’ cross-modal interactions is important for understanding both cross-modal processing and human awareness. Regarding cross-modal processing,

(57)

unconscious unisensory processing (e.g., visual) is limited mainly to the earliest stages of sensory processing (e.g., the primary visual cortex, Sterzer, Stein, Ludwig, Rothkirch, & Hesselmann, 2014). Thus, any demonstration of cross-modal interactions occurring outside of unisensory awareness would imply that primary sensory cortices should not be considered as uniquely unisensory but rather as areas that receive inputs from other sensory modalities. As for models of human awareness, cross-modal interactions outside of unisensory awareness have important theoretical implications because it would challenge the proposal that cross-modal binding requires conscious processing of the unisensory stimuli (Baars, 2002).

Two psychophysical paradigms are particularly efficient in inducing interocular suppression: binocular rivalry, and its variant continuous flash suppression (CFS). Binocular rivalry occurs when unrelated images are presented at the same time onto homologous portions of the two retinae. The conflicting dichoptic stimulation prevents binocular integration and triggers competing interocular inhibition processes, resulting in alternating suppression of each eye and thus fluctuations in visual perception between the two monocular images (Alais & Blake, 2005; Blake & Logothetis, 2002). CFS occurs when one eye is presented with a static

(58)

visual stimulus, while the other one is presented with a dynamic mask in which random high-contrast patterns (known as “mondrians”) are flashed sequentially, typically at a frequency of 10 Hz (Tsuchiya and Koch, 2005). Dynamic patterns are known to predominate during binocular rivalry and CFS exploits this characteristic using flickering Mondrians to produce strong suppressive power and consequently long-lasting periods of suppression compared with binocular rivalry (Tsuchiya, Koch, Gilroy, & Blake, 2006). An advantage of CFS is that while suppression epochs in binocular rivalry are highly variable in duration and the dominant percept at onset is unpredictable, CFS allows a more predictable suppression: if no transients occur in the eye presented with the static image, the dynamic mask will immediately suppress the other stimulus for several seconds.

Evidence from several recent studies has shown that during either binocular rivalry or CFS, non-visual sensory modalities (e.g., auditory; Conrad, Bartels, Kleiner, & Noppeney, 2010; Lunghi, Morrone, & Alais, 2014, haptic; Lunghi & Alais, 2013, 2015; Lunghi, Binda, & Morrone, 2010; Lunghi & Morrone, 2013; Lunghi et al., 2014, olfactory; Zhou, Jiang, He, & Chen, 2010, proprioceptive; Salomon, Lim, Herbelin, Hesselmann, & Blanke, 2013, vestibular; Salomon, Kaliuzhna, Herbelin, & Blanke, 2015, and bodily; Saletu, Anderer, Kinsperger, Gru¨ nberger, & Sieghart, 1988,

(59)

signals) are able to interact with the suppressed visual stimulus (reviewed in Deroy et al., 2016). In particular, it has been shown that during binocular rivalry, unambiguous haptic stimulation promotes dominance of the congruent visual stimulus independently of awareness: touch prolongs dominance durations of the congruent visual stimulus and is also able to rescue it from suppression, making it visible (Lunghi et al., 2010). This cross-modal interaction is highly specific and relies on the objective congruency in spatial frequency (Lunghi et al., 2010), orientation (Lunghi & Alais, 2013), and spatial proximity (Lunghi & Morrone, 2013) between the visual image and the tactile stimulus. A study investigating the effect of touch on the strength of binocular rivalry suppression using visual and tactile gratings showed that congruent haptic stimulation interacted with binocular rivalry mainly by preventing the congruent visual stimulus from becoming deeply suppressed (Lunghi & Alais, 2015). While this suggests the cross-modal interaction involves strengthening the congruent visual stimulus, a complementary possibility yet to be investigated is that the tactile stimulus may also suppress the incongruent visual stimulus. Even though these two mechanisms are not mutually exclusive, binocular rivalry paradigms (unlike CFS) cannot disentangle their relative contribution to the observed effect. Understanding this is particularly

(60)

important for assessing whether the integration between visual and haptic signals occurs at the conscious level (through the suppression of the incongruent dominant stimulus) or unconscious level (through the enhancement of the suppressed congruent visual signal).

Here, we investigate whether haptic stimulation can interact with a visual grating stimulus rendered invisible by CFS. Using a tactile grating stimulus, we will test whether the interaction with the suppressed visual grating is tuned for matching spatial frequency and orientation. On the basis of the results obtained in binocular rivalry paradigms, we anticipate that haptic gratings, when congruent with orientation and spatial frequency of the visual grating, will decrease the suppression duration of the visual grating.

(61)

2.2 MATERIALS AND METHODS

2.2.1 Participants

A total of 14 subjects (3 females, average age ± SD: 25.9 ± 3.5 years), including two authors of this study (C. L., and L. L. V.), participated in the experiment. All subjects had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, no strong eye preference (measured as eye-predominance in binocular rivalry), normal stereoacuity (Frisby Stereotest, Sasieni, 1978) and were right-handed. Except the two authors, all subjects were naïve to the purposes of the experiment.

2.2.2 Ethic Statement

The experimental protocol was approved by the Tuscany regional ethics committee of the Azienda Ospedaliero-Universitaria Meyer and was performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants gave written informed consent.

2.2.3 Apparatus and Stimuli

The experiment took place in a quiet room in total darkness to remove any spatial frame of reference. The visual stimuli were developed

(62)

in Matlab (version 7.11.0, The MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA) using Psychtoolbox-3 (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997) running on a PC. The stimuli consisted of red or blue dynamic maskers (“Mondrians”, Hesselmann & Malach, 2011) and red or blue target gratings. The stimuli were presented dichoptically through anaglyph red and blue goggles, superimposed with the grating in the center of the Mondrian, and were presented on a uniform black background (luminance: 0.17 cd/m2) in central vision,

(63)

Figure 2.1. Experimental paradigm. (a) Diagram of a visual trial: 500 ms after

the onset of the cue (letter ‘‘V’’), the visual stimuli appeared. The contrast of the target grating was ramped from 0 to 10%, 20%, or 40% to avoid any transient that could break CFS. When the grating emerged from interocular suppression, observers reported its orientation (tilted left or tilted right) by pressing the appropriate mouse

(64)

button. (b) Diagram of a visuo-haptic trial: same as (a), except that after the presentation of the cue observers were instructed to reach the haptic grating with their right index finger and explore it until the visual grating became visible. The visual stimuli appeared 500 ms after the onset of touch. On each trial, the orientation of the haptic grating was varied and could be either congruent or incongruent with the orientation of the visual target grating.

Riferimenti

Documenti correlati

Table 3 – Giving-up densities GUDs, number of visits, duration of visits, proportion of time spent foraging and in vigilance in relation cognitive style, experimental day, sex and

It is well known that the distribution of the size of packets and self-similarity of the input traffic have an impact on the transmission quality of service (determined by

Unperturbed viscous dynamics We now aim to discuss a model in order to build the motion equations of a spherically symmetric and uniform gas cloud, including the corrections due to

Second component gathers items referring to discriminatory behaviors other-directed (Express themselves negatively about gay people in general other than myself, Use

Anche se l'oblio scaturisce dalla problematica della memoria e della fedeltà al passato e il perdono da quella della colpa e della riconciliazione con il passato,

Stephens Page 1 Per quasi due anni, il fidanzato di Kiera, Denny, è stato tutto quello che lei ha sempre desiderato: amorevole, tenero, completamente dedito a lei.. Quando

The three parenting dimensions of positive parental involvement, autonomy support and provision of structure - highlighted by Self-determination theory as closely linked to the

 the proximity of this opportunity to the features under development. All above brought to a perceived reduction of team’s capability to absorb new product knowledge. On