• Non ci sono risultati.

Vibration effect on ball score test in international vs. national level table tennis

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Condividi "Vibration effect on ball score test in international vs. national level table tennis"

Copied!
6
0
0

Testo completo

(1)

Biologyof Sport, Vol. 35 No4, 2018

329

INTRODUCTION

In table tennis (TT), motor skills are crucial for discriminating a player’s level: a high skill level is characterized by accurate, fast movements [1]. Better players exhibit higher general physiological outcomes, such as higher maximal oxygen consumption, and per-formance indicators, such as maximal accumulated oxygen deficit and total amount of anaerobic work [2], hence achieving better scores. A low level athlete can compete with a high level athlete for only a few minutes, then he/she fails to keep up with the opponent’s pace, due to his/her poorer timing capability [3]. In fact, the TT ball travels at ~27 m·s-1 and TT players have only ~1 s to get ready for

the return after the opponent hits the ball [3]. The athlete’s overall ability emerges especially under fatigued conditions [4], mainly in terms of fewer errors made.

The ability to be accurate and fast requires superior neuromus-cular control of movement, especially of the upper limbs, given the

Vibration effect on ball score test in international vs. national

level table tennis

AUTHORS: Luca P. Ardigò1, Antonio Dello Iacono2,3, Alessandro M. Zagatto4, Nicola L. Bragazzi5,6, Goran Kuvacic7, Marianna Bellafiore8, Johnny Padulo7,9

1 School of Exercise and Sport Science, Department of Neurosciences, Biomedicine and Movement Sciences,

University of Verona, Verona, Italy

2 Zinman College of Physical Education and Sport Sciences, Wingate Institute, Netanya, Israel

3 Institute of Clinical Exercise and Health Science, School of Health and Life Sciences, University of the West of

Scotland, Hamilton, UK

4 Laboratory of Physiology and Sport Performance (LAFIDE), Faculty of Sciences, Department of Physical Education,

Univ Estadual Paulista-UNESP, Bauru – SP, Brazil

5 School of Public Health, Department of Health Sciences (DISSAL), Genoa University, Genoa, Italy

6 Department of Neuroscience, Rehabilitation, Ophthalmology, Genetics, Maternal and Child Health (DINOGMI),

Section of Psychiatry, Genoa University, Genoa, Italy

7 Faculty of Kinesiology, University of Split, Split, Croatia

8 Sport and Exercise Science Research Unit, Department of Psychological, Pedagogical and Educational Sciences,

University of Palermo, Palermo, Italy

9 University eCampus, Novedrate, Italy

ABSTRACT: In table tennis, motor skills are crucial for discriminating player level. However, there is a dearth of studies exploring the impact of a vibrational stimulus on performance. Thirty-four male players (age 25±2 years; body mass index, BMI 23.4±1.2 kg·m-2) participated in the study. Seventeen played at international level (IL),

while the remaining 17 played at national level (NL). The participants underwent a ball-handling test, the ball score, before (PRE) and after (POST) a vibrational stimulus. Intra-class correlation (ICC) for the ball score result showed good reliability (ICC 0.87 for IL and 0.80 for NL). Repeated measures ANOVA showed differences between groups for ball score (p=0.000) and a significant group×time interaction (p=0.004). Better performances were observed for the IL group than for the NL group, significantly only for POST. Vibration produced positive and negative effects in IL and NL groups, respectively.

CITATION: Ardigò LP, Iacono AD, Zagatto AM et al. Vibration effect on ball score test in international vs. national level table tennis. Biol Sport. 2018;35(4):329–334.

Received: 2017-12-10; Reviewed: 2018-04-20; Re-submitted: 2018-05-21; Accepted: 2018-06-14; Published: 2018-08-31.

100-200-ms visuomotor delay [5]. However, there are no studies on TT investigating the factors discriminating the elite athlete from the amateur one. Furthermore, there are no studies concerning simple and practical tests that are reliable and ecologically valid and enable to evaluate TT players during game play.

It is well known that there exists a strong relationship between neuromuscular response and peak force and rate of force develop-ment [6]. A vibration of the upper limb, which is a mechanical stimulus characterized by oscillatory motion [7], may affect the neu-romuscular response and, consequently, have an impact on perfor-mance in terms of muscle activation, force, and power [8]. In more detail, vibration is believed to act via neurogenic potentiation involv-ing spinal reflexes and stretch-reflex muscle-spindle activity, which is based on the tonic vibration reflex [9]. For instance, in the young population, increased surface electromyography (sEMG) and

Key words: Muscle activity Motor control Reaction time Fatigued conditions Corresponding author: Johnny Padulo

University eCampus, Novedrate, Italy

Via Isimbardi, 10, 22060, Novedrate, Italy

Telephone: +393477691228 E-mail: sportcinetic@gmail.com

(2)

The participants refrained from drinking alcohol or caffeine for 24 hours, and fasted for at least 4 hours before testing, to reduce any interference with results. Each participant completed all trials on the testing days at the same time to eliminate any circadian variation. Both groups were homogeneous with regard to training status. None of the participants underwent any strenuous endurance activity or resistance training outside the training protocol.

After being informed of procedures, methods, benefits and pos-sible risks of the study, each participant read and signed an informed consent form, in accordance with the requirements of the Declaration of Helsinki. The university human ethics committee followed the ethical standards for human studies and approved all experimental procedures.

Procedures

This experimental study was approached through a “cross-over” observational design. We used vibrational stimulus and group as the independent variables and ball score result as the dependent variable. Data were acquired over two different days, from 9:00 a.m. until 1:00 p.m., at average temperature of 23°C (range 22-24°C). Four participants (1 IL and 3 NL) were left-handed. Each participant wore typical TT sportswear. All experiments were done at the “Table Ten-nis International Centre” in Cagliari (Italy). Assessment was done over three different steps (Table 2, day 1) and repeated one week later (Table 2, day 2), to assess the reliability of the measures.

A standardized protocol was used to weigh the participants [12]: body mass was measured twice to the nearest 0.1 kg on an elec-tronic scale (Personal Line, Filizola, Campo Grande, MS, Brazil) and the mean value was taken. Height was measured twice to the near-est 0.01 m using a portable stadiometer (67310, Country Technol-ogy, Gays Mills, WI, USA) and the mean value was taken.

Ball score

The ball score result was measured as the number of ball contacts with the racket (i.e., with frequent low vertical bounces) in 15 s. In the authors’ opinion, managing to achieve with the racket a high number of ball contacts per time unit reasonably testifies to a player’s great ball-handling capacity. Each participant attended a whole 1-hour preliminary learning session and was tested on an official TT table after performing a standardized warm-up (7 min with an opponent throwing the ball to the participant and the participant freely return-ing the ball with both forehand and backhand strokes). The experiment started with the participant standing with a 90° elbow angle and the ball lying on the racket. The test was performed three times, and the mean score was used for further analysis. The procedure was never interrupted during the tests and no injuries were reported.

This test session was performed within a 2×1-m area, used as acquisition volume, and recorded with a high-speed camera (Exilim FH20, Casio, Japan) located on a 1.5-m high tripod at 3 m from the participant, so as to be perpendicular to the sagittal plane of the ball trajectory.

voluntary activation, following a mechanical vibration, can be ob-served [10]. Therefore, vibration could enable one to discriminate subjects with a high nerve transmission potential from those with a low one.

Despite a dearth of studies on the matter, a mechanical vibration of the upper limb has already been shown to increase muscle activa-tion acutely, especially in internaactiva-tional-level TT players [11]. Padulo et al. investigated muscle activation by recording sEMG root mean square from upper arm muscles during vibration and maximal vol-untary contraction (for normalization purposes [11]). Increased muscle activation is a clear requirement for performance improvement.

Therefore, given the already proven relationship between me-chanical vibration and muscle activation, the aim of this study was to investigate with a ball-handling test (ball score) – as a proxy for real game play – the effect on TT performance of a vibrational stim-ulus in international and national TT players. Ball handling is clear-ly an essential skill for TT players, and both TT players and their coaches could take great advantage from an ecologically reliable ball-handling test. A ball score measure was assessed for reliability, as well.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Subjects

For sample size calculation purposes, a pilot study that included a sample of 17 subjects was performed a priori and gave a statistical power greater than 0.81. Thirty-four male TT players participated in this study (age 25±2 years; body mass 70.3±4.4 kg, height 174±6 cm, and body mass index (BMI) 23.4±1.2 kg·m-2).

Seven-teen players with international competition experience were allo-cated to the international-level group (all within the world top 50, training six days per week, five hours per day, training experience 12.2±4.8 years; IL), while the remaining 17 without international competition experience were allocated to the national-level group (ranked within regional rank, training three days per week, maximum two hours per day, training experience 6.6±4.9 years; NL; Table 1). Group-shared inclusion criteria were: 1) having competed during the previous season, and 2) possession of a valid medical clearance for competition. The participants were healthy, without any muscular, neurological and/or tendinous injuries, and were not taking any drug.

TABLE 1. Anthropometric characteristics.

Group (yrs)Age Height (cm) Mass (kg) (kg·mBMI -2)

IL 24.4±2.3 172±8 63.1±4.5* 21.4±1.5*

NL 25.9±1.6 175±4 77.5±4.3 25.3±0.8

Data (means±SD) for all subjects broken down for international (IL=17) and national (NL=17) levels. BMI=body mass index, * = statistically significant with p<0.05.

(3)

Biologyof Sport, Vol. 35 No4, 2018

331

Athletes were taught to perform the technical task by means of

a  video reproducing the test, with instructions provided by a qualified coach. The camera-sampling rate was fixed at 420 Hz (1 frame=420 Hz, i.e., lasting ~0.0024”) and the resolution at 224×168 for a spatial precision of ±5 pixels. All the movies were analyzed off-line by the same operator and by using motion analysis software (Dartfish 5.5Pro, Dartfish, Fribourg, Switzerland). The mea-sured variable was the number of ball contacts in 15 s.

Upper limb vibration

The vibrating equipment used was the Nemes Arms(nemeS Han-dleMed, Bosco System, Norway), with the frequency set at 40 Hz, a peak-to-peak displacement of about 2.2 mm and an acceleration of about 7.7 g for 30 s [8]. During the vibrational stimulus, the participants assumed a standing position, with the dominant arm flexed at 90° and the hand in a neutral position while gripping the handlebar (Figure 1).

Statistical analysis

Data were reported as mean±standard deviation (SD). The assump-tion of normality was verified with the Shapiro-Wilk test. To assess

the reliability of the ball score measures the intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC) was performed [13]. The ICC represents a measure of relative reliability showing the degree to which the sample’s indi-vidual measures keep their value with repeated measurements. An ICC higher than 0.75 generally exhibits excellent reliability [14]. Also, the coefficient of variation (CV) was computed.

ANOVA with a grouping factor and with time as a repeated mea-sure was used to assess the effect of vibration on the ball score result. The normality of distribution, the homogeneity of covariance matrices, the independence and the sphericity assumptions were verified. Mauchly’s test was used to check for sphericity. If the sphericity assumption was violated, the Greenhouse-Geisser correction was applied to adjust for the degrees of freedom of the interaction effect between different time points and different sample groups. If the value was less than 0.75, the Huynh-Feldt correction was used. Post-hoc tests were performed using Bonferroni correction. The effect size (ES) was classified as small (0.01<η2<0.06), medium

(0.06<η2<0.14) or large (η2>0.14 [15]). The significance level

was set at p<0.05 and all the statistical analyses were performed with the commercial software SPSS for Windows (version 23.00, IBM, Chicago, IL, USA).

Receiver operating characteristics (ROC) is a technique that allows the visualization of test performance in terms of sensitivity and spec-ificity [16]. ROC analysis is mostly used to evaluate a classifier based on data collected under similar conditions, but can be used as a binary classifier system in different conditions, as well, in that their discrimination thresholds are varied [17]. We computed ROC analy-sis calculating the area under the curve (AUC), in order to obtain stimulus-specific (PRE vs. POST) or group-specific (IL vs. NL) cut-off values. All pairwise comparisons (namely between IL and NL athletes, TABLE 2. Experimental design.

day 1

ball score 3’ pause upper limb vibration 3’ pause ball score day 2

ball score 3’ pause upper limb vibration 3’ pause ball score

Figure 1. Schematic sketch of the subject’s posture for maximal isometric voluntary contraction and vibration interventions [11].

Figure 2. Mean±standard deviation ball score results before and after vibrational stimulus in the international-level group (IL) and national-level group (NL). “#” p<0.01 between post- and pre-vibrational stimulus. “*” p<0.05 between post- and pre-pre-vibrational stimulus for each group and between groups in post-vibrational stimulus as well.

(4)

17% and 14%, respectively. Repeated measures ANOVA showed differences between groups for ball score (F(1,32)=24.27, p=0.000,

η2=0.45, ES=large, Figure 2) and a significant group×time

interaction (F(1,32)=9.54, p=0.004, η2=0.24, ES=large), both

dur-ing the first and the second bouts.

Correcting for multiple comparisons, the comparison between IL and NL groups after vibration (POST) showed a statistically significant difference. Vibration had a significant effect on the ball score result in both groups. Vibration significantly improved the IL ball score result, while it significantly worsened the NL ball score result, mak-ing the POST inter-group difference significant. These findmak-ings were confirmed by the multi-ROC analysis (overall multi-AUC=0.69, Fig-ure 3).

PRE and POST) were computed to approximate the multi-class ROC, correcting for multiple comparisons. The Youden J index was calcu-lated to separate the different groups for each pairwise comparison with acceptable sensitivity and specificity, using commercial software, namely MedCalc Statistical Software version 16.4.3 (MedCalc Soft-ware bvba, Ostend, Belgium). Finally, multi-class ROC analysis was performed using the pROC package run in the R environment. RESULTS

No significant differences were found between the two groups for age (p=0.09) and body height (p=0.42), while NL players had higher values for body mass and BMI (p=0.000; Table 1).

Ball score measures’ ICC showed good reliability in both groups (ICC 0.87 and 0.80 for IL and NL, respectively). The CV values were

Figure 3. Multiclass receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analysis for evaluating the potential of the ball-handling test in distinguishing between before (PRE) and after (POST) vibration and between the international-level group (IL) and national-level group (NL). “*” p<0.05, correcting for multiple comparisons.

(5)

Biologyof Sport, Vol. 35 No4, 2018

333

DISCUSSION

The main findings were that: (1) IL resulted in better performances compared to NL, at both PRE (not significantly, p=0.24) and POST (significantly) time points; and (2) the vibrational stimulus produced positive and negative effects on the ball score results of IL and NL groups, respectively.

One point should be made regarding the non-significant PRE ball score result difference between IL and NL groups before proceeding with the discussion of the results. As a matter for further research, a TT ball-handling test more demanding than ball score may better highlight performance differences between different players’ levels. It was found that motor efficiency produces athletic skills character-ized by automaticity, speed, and accuracy [1]. Our study demonstrates that the ability to achieve a high ball score result was mostly related to the athlete’s level of expertise. Experts in highly reactive sports such as TT move rapidly, effortlessly, and smoothly [1,18]. In the context of athletic performance, elite karate athletes require propor-tionally less brain activation in task-relevant brain areas (i.e., cortical dorsal and “mirror” pathways) compared to amateur athletes or non-athletes (the “neural efficiency” hypothesis, as postulated from the electroencephalographic investigation carried out by Babiloni et al. [19]). Motor efficiency can be achieved through intensive training, leading to improved perception, focus, anticipation, planning, and fast responses [18]. The relationship between the ball score results and the level of expertise is, therefore, logical, considering that the task consisted of finely structured movements of the upper extremi-ties, with minimal variations of the complex hand-racket movement and the ball trajectory. Due to the high accuracy required, even small deviations from an ideal ball trajectory can disrupt the course of action, requiring corrections that may have detrimental effects on the task. In addition, optimal intra-limb coordination patterns are pro-moted in the acquisition of expertise [20]. An “ideal motor pattern” should minimize intra-cyclic speed variations and stability of the demanded tasks. Accordingly, IL players, both at PRE and POST,

presented less variability compared to the NL group (Figure 4). The more stable, less variable performance in the IL group highlights the superior ability in using a more efficient motor control pattern in relation to the interacting constraints related to the (technical) task, (physiological) organism, and (vibration) conditioning stimulus.

Another possible explanation could be related to the different capacities in the learning process of the task among the subjects. Learning a movement skill implies an information transfer from teach-ers to learnteach-ers, commonly carried out via observation (observational learning [21,22]). Previous studies have reported differences between elite and non-elite athletes in exploiting observational learning, with the elite athletes using this function more than the non-elite athletes. Observational learning functions have a cognitive aspect and the performance function (one of the three factors of the Functions of Observational Learning Questionnaire [23]) has a motivational aspect. Since the cognitive functions are correlated with psychological factors, such as athletic self-confidence [24], this is one of the reasons for the supremacy of these athletes and their success in reaching an elite level. Elite athletes also use performance function to a much greater extent. This could depend on their previous experiences in using observational learning as a factor for motivational enhance-ment [25].

The second finding was that the local vibrational stimulus produced opposite outcomes in the task, with positive and negative effects on the ball score results of IL and NL players, respectively. Padulo et al. [11], investigating the effects of local muscle vibration on the sEMG responses of the forearm and hand muscles among TT athletes of different competition levels, found that larger muscle activation could be skill-level related. Indeed, the application of local muscle vibration may be crucial for individualizing the vibration load relative to the skill level of athletes. The specific nature of the vibratory effect could be dependent on the specificity of the stimulus of the skill training [26]. In fact, the designed experimental exercise, matching similar biodynamic responses in the kinetic chain system (wrist-metacarpal and finger joint kinematics) commonly involved in TT skills, resulted in positive effects on the ball score performance. As reported by Padulo et al. [11], the application of local muscle vibra-tion during a hand-racket complex configuravibra-tion, similar to that used in our study, resulted in higher activation of most of the target mus-cles in IL compared to NL athletes [27]. From a functional perspec-tive, higher activation of muscle groups may result in the greater ability of high-skilled athletes to apply transfer processes, leading to better performance. Moreover, the pattern activation acquired during skill learning processes or isolated conditioning exercises could be refined, leading to changes in the connectivity between these periph-eral effectors and the neural structures and in the motor cortex plas-ticity. The motor cortex plasticity results in an alteration of the corti-cal synapse number, synaptic strength, and topography of stimulation-evoked movement representations. In fact, the discrete movements of TT players across different joints are controlled by the primary motor cortex [28]. Therefore, the concurrent application of Figure 4. Scores of the ball-handling test in the international-level

group (IL) and national-level group (NL) of table tennis players, broken down for the test’s three different 5-s bouts.

(6)

better performances compared to those of NL players after vibration. Vibration produced positive and negative effects in IL and NL players, respectively.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank all the players who volunteered in this study. No external financial support was received.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they do not have any conflict of interest. local muscle vibration during skill learning processes or conditioning

exercises may produce a potentiation effect for functional/structural adaptations of both the peripheral effectors and the central neural structures. However, we acknowledge that the vibration worsening effect on NL performance remains still surprising. One hypothesis for that might be related to some still undisclosed nervous conduction difference between IL and NL athletes. Such a relevant issue may be worth further electrophysiological investigation going beyond the methods of the present study.

CONCLUSIONS

The aim of this research was to investigate the effect of a vibra-tional stimulus on a table tennis test result. IL players’ results showed

1. Yarrow K, Brown P, Krakauer JW. Inside the brain of an elite athlete: the neural processes that support high

achievement in sports. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(8):585-596. 2. Zagatto AM, Gobatto CA. Relationship

between anaerobic parameters provided from MAOD and critical power model in specific table tennis test. Int J Sports Med. 2012;33(8):613-620.

3. Ak E, Kocak S. Coincidence-anticipation timing and reaction time in youth tennis and table tennis players. Percept Mot Skills. 2010;110(3 Pt 1):879-887. 4. Aune TK, Ingvaldsen RP, Ettema GJ.

Effect of physical fatigue on motor control at different skill levels. Percept Mot Skills. 2008;106(2):371-386. 5. Van Soest AJ, Bobbert MF, Van Ingen

Schenau GJ. A control strategy for the execution of explosive movements from varying starting positions. J

Neurophysiol. 1994;71(4):1390-1402. 6. Sale DG. Neural adaptation to resistance

training. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1988;20(5 Suppl):S135-S145. 7. Cardinale M, Wakeling J. Whole body

vibration exercise: are vibrations good for you? Br J Sports Med. 2005; 39(9):585-9.

8. Bosco C, Cardinale M, Tsarpela O. Influence of vibration on mechanical power and electromyogram activity in human arm flexor muscles. Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol. 1999; 79(4):306-311.

9. Cochrane DJ. Vibration exercise: the potential benefits. Int J Sports Med. 2011;32(2):75-99.

10. Souron R, Besson T, Millet GY, Lapole T. Acute and chronic neuromuscular adaptations to local vibration training. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2017;

117(10):1939-1964. 11. Padulo J, Di Giminiani R, Dello

Iacono A, Zagatto AM, Migliaccio GM, Grgantov Z, Ardigò LP. Lower Arm Muscle Activation during

Indirect-Localized Vibration: The Influence of Skill Levels When Applying Different Acceleration Loads. Front Physiol. 2016;7:242.

12. Lohman T, Roche A, Martorell R. Anthropometric standardization reference manual, 2nd ed. Champaign:

Human kinetics; 1991.

13. Hopkins WG. Measures of reliability in sports medicine and science. Sports Med. 2000;30(1):1-15.

14. Fleiss GL. The measurement of interrater agreement. In: Statistical methods for rates and proportions, 3rd edition.

Fleiss GL, Levin B, Paik MC, editors. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.; 2003. p. 598-626.

15. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988. 16. McCall A, Fanchini M, Coutts AJ.

Prediction: The Modern-Day Sport-Science and Sports-Medicine “Quest for the Holy Grail”. Int J Sports Physiol Perform. 2017;12(5):704-706. 17. Mathworks [Internet]. Detector

Performance Analysis Using ROC Curves. 2018. [cited 2018 May 21]. Available from: http://www.mathworks. com/help/phased/examples/detector-performance-analysis-using-roc-curves. html?requestedDomain=www. mathworks.com&nocookie=true 18. Wolf S, Brölz E, Scholz D, Ramos-Murguialday A, Keune PM, Hautzinger M, Birbaumer N, Strehl U. Winning the game: brain processes in expert, young elite and amateur table tennis players. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014;8:370.

19. Babiloni C, Marzano N, Infarinato F, Iacoboni M, Rizza G, Aschieri P, Cibelli G, Soricelli A, Eusebi F, Del Percio C. “Neural efficiency” of experts’ brain during judgment of actions: a high-resolution EEG study in elite and amateur karate athletes. Behav Brain Res. 2010;207(2):466-475.

20. Seifert L, Komar J, Barbosa T, Toussaint H, Millet G, Davids K. Coordination pattern variability provides functional adaptations to constraints in swimming performance. Sports Med. 2014;44(10):1333-1345.

21. McCullagh P, Davis MR. Modelling: Considerations for motor skill performance and psychological responses. In: Singer RN,

Hausenblas HA, Janelle CM, editors. Handbook of sport psychology. New York: Wiley; 2001. p. 205-238. 22. McCullagh P, Meyer KN. Learning versus

correct models: influence of model type on the learning of a free-weight squat lift. Res Q Exerc Sport. 1997; 68(1):56-61.

23. Cumming J, Clark SE, Ste-Marie DM, Law B, Ramsey R, Murphy L. The functions of observational learning questionnaire (FOLQ). Psychol Sport Exerc. 2005;6(5):517-537. 24. Hall CR, Munroe-Chandler KJ,

Cumming J, Law B, Ramsey R, Murphy L. Imagery and observational learning use and their relationship to sport confidence. J Sports Sci. 2009; 27(4):327-337.

25. Bandura A. Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall Inc; 1986.

26. Pearce AJ, Thickbroom GW, Byrnes ML, Mastaglia FL. Functional reorganisation of the corticomotor projection to the hand in skilled racquet players. Exp Brain Res. 2000;130(2):238-243. 27. Mischi M, Cardinale M. The effects of a

28-Hz vibration on arm muscle activity during isometric exercise. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2009;41(3):645-653. 28. Adkins DL, Boychuk J, Remple MS,

Kleim JA. Motor training induces experience-specific patterns of plasticity across motor cortex and spinal cord. J Appl Physiol (1985). 2006; 101(6):1776-1782.

Riferimenti

Documenti correlati

It should be noted, that the decline in c, k, f o values can be undesirable because it can lead to the growth of tool deflections during milling (induced by the decrease of

Un giocatore non può colpire due volte consecutive la palla e una squadra ha al massimo la possibilità di fare tre tocchi prima di inviarla nel campo avversario...

When in normal conditions (i.e. no failures), packets entering the network are labeled with tag=0 and routed through their primary path (Fig. To detect failures, SPIDER does not rely

dia ambientale | 2016 | 90 SOSTENIBILIT Á DAVIDE BATTE GOLIA: L’UMILTÀ DEL SUOLO AGRICOLO E L’ARROGANZA URBANISTICA Paolo Pileri La sentenza 2921 del Consiglio di Stato del 28

La resistenza delineata da Rolando è dunque la premessa dottrinale dell’idea duecentesca di una tutela sovrana dei deboli ad opera della legge, che esalta la legalità di

For the RC FLATSLAB structure, the first reference earthquake on the unprotected building introduced some damage (which changed its frequency) while the

Architetto, ricercatore a tempo definito in Tecnologie dell’Architettura presso la Facoltà di Architettura dell’Università degli Studi di Firenze, docente del Master di II Livello

Nel notare il valido apporto dell’aria nella organizzazione dell’habitat dell’uomo, ci chiediamo ancora come sia realmente possibile dimenticare l’importante ruolo che un