• Non ci sono risultati.

Accuracy of Rats in Discriminating Visual Objects Is Explained by the Complexity of Their Perceptual Strategy

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Condividi "Accuracy of Rats in Discriminating Visual Objects Is Explained by the Complexity of Their Perceptual Strategy"

Copied!
8
0
0

Testo completo

(1)

Supplemental Information

Accuracy of Rats in Discriminating Visual

Objects Is Explained by the Complexity

of Their Perceptual Strategy

Vladimir Djurdjevic, Alessio Ansuini, Daniele Bertolini, Jakob H. Macke, and Davide Zoccolan

(2)

A

B

#1 #2 #3 #4 #5 #6 #1 #2 #3 #4 #5 #6 #1 #2 #3 #4 #5 #6 L C R Reference object (the “tripod”)

Trigger Presentation Response

C ? R

Time

Distractor object

Trigger Presentation Response

C ? L

Time

C

Figure S1. Experimental rig and behavioral task, Related to Figure 1 and STAR Methods. (A-B) The drawings illustrate the operant box in which rats were trained to perform the visual

discrimination task described in our study. In the pictures, several elements of the rig are visible: 1) the box (whose walls were actually black, but are rendered here as transparent to allow viewing the inside of the box); 2) the block holding the three feeding needles, which served both as touch sensors, to record rat responses, and reward ports, to deliver liquid reward in case of correct choices; 3) the viewing hole, where a rat had to insert its head to face the stimulus display and interact with the touch sensors; and 4) the computer monitor, where the visual stimuli were shown. Note that six of such boxes were simultaneously active during the experiment, so as to train in parallel all the rats tested in our study.

(C) Schematic of the object discrimination task. The animals learned to trigger stimulus presentation

by licking the central sensor and to associate each object identity to a specific reward port/sensor – the right-side port was associated to the reference tripod object, while the left-side port was associated to all the distractor objects (distractor #10 is shown here as an example).

(3)

0.12 0.16 0.2 0.24 0.35 0.4 0.45 0.5 0.55 0.28 r = 0.99 (***)

Fraction of tripod responses (ptripod)

rat 1 rat 2 rat 3 rat 4 rat 5 rat 6

Logloss’ upper bound (fixed prob. model)

Figure S2. The logloss’ upper bound depends on the predictability of rat responses, Related to Figure 4.

Relationship between the logloss of the constant-probability response model (used to estimate the loss’ upper bound) and the fraction of tripod responses that each rat gave to the random tripods. The two quantities were strongly and significantly correlated (two-tailed t-test; p < 0.001).

(4)

0.2 0.6 1 1.4 1.8 0 0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 3.5 r = 0.975 (***) -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 r = 0.61 (***)

Predicted distractor evidence0 1 2 3 4 -1 Measured distractor evidence rat 1 rat 2 rat 3 rat 4 rat 5 rat 6 #8 #9 #10 #11 #5 #4 #6 #3 #2 #1 #7 distractor

Predicted distractor evidence

Measured

distractor evidence

Figure S3. Predicting rat perceptual choices to the distractor objects, Related to Figure 4 and STAR Methods.

Relationship between measured and predicted distractor evidence, as obtained: 1) by considering all rats and distractor conditions together (left); and 2) after averaging, separately for each animal, the measured and predicted evidences across the 11 distractors (right; dots show means ± SEM). Both correlations were significant according to a two-tailed t-test (p < 0.001).

(5)

0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 15 20 25 30 35 Size 0.9 1.1 1.3 1.5 0.7 15 20 25 30 35 Size P(choice = tripod) P(choice = distractor)

fixed-template model predictions rat choices

Figure S4. A fixed-size, template-matching strategy does not explain rat discrimination performance with the reference and distractor objects across size changes, Related to Figure 5.

Rat group average accuracy (black) with the tripod (left) and the five hardest distractors (right) as a function of size is compared to the average accuracy yielded by the logistic regression models (red), based on the classification images obtained from the regular-size (30º) random tripods (dots show means over the six rats ± SEM). The curves (that were normalized to their values at 30º for a better comparison) all displayed some degree of modulation over the size axis, but this was much sharper for the model predictions than for rat performances. Such different modulation was statistically assessed by a two-way ANOVA with size and observer (i.e., either rat or model) as factors. For both the left and right plots, the main effect of size was significant (p < 0.001; F8,40 = 175.474 and F8,40 = 46.565 respectively), as well as its interaction with observer (p < 0.001; F8,40 = 22.387 and F8,40 = 14.993 respectively), thus showing that the two performances dropped at a different pace along the size axis. This, in turn, confirmed the failure of the fixed-size template-matching model to account for rat size-tolerant behavior.

(6)

0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8

Correlation between measured and

predicted distractor evidence

late

early early late early late early late 25º(size) 30º(size) 25º(size) 30º(size)

predictions based on regular-size templates

predictions based on rescaled small-size templates

ns ns

Figure S5. Transformation-tolerance of rat perceptual strategy with the distractor objects, Related to Figure 4 and Figure 6.

The correlation between measured and predicted distractor evidence across the 11 distractors was computed, for each rat, using the logistic regression model (eq. 1). The resulting correlation coeffi-cients were averaged to yield the bars shown here (mean over 6 rats ± SEM). The predictions were derived from models based either on the classification images obtained at the regular size (i.e., those shown in Figure 6A) or on the rescaled versions of the classification images obtained at the small size (i.e., those shown in Figure 6C) – left and right panels, respectively. Correlations were obtained for the distractors shown both at size 25º and 30º, in both the early (black) and late (gray) sessions, i.e., when the random tripods were shown, respectively, either at the regular or small size (see STAR Methods). No pairwise comparisons were significant according to a two-tailed, paired t-test. This indicates that the perceptual template obtained at a given size (e.g., 30º) was equally good at accounting for rat choices at that size and at a different one (e.g., 25º), once properly rescaled, thus confirming the tolerance of the object processing strategy to size changes.

(7)

Rat 1 Rat 2 Rat 3 Rat 4 Rat 5 Rat 6 0.25 0.3 0.35 0.4 0.45 0.5 0.55 0.6 0.65 0.7 1st 2nd 3th 4th rat 1 rat 2 rat 3 rat 4 rat 5 rat 6 Logloss Trials’ set 40 30 20 10 0 * within

rat betweenrat

Euclidean distance

Similarity of the classification images (in terms of predicted discriminability

of the distractors)

C

Figure S6. Stability of rat perceptual strategies over time, Related to Figure 6 and STAR Meth-ods.

(A) The figure shows, for each rat, the classification images obtained in four consecutive, equally

sized sets of trials (see STAR Methods for details). This allows appreciating how, for most animals, the pattern of salient and anti-salient features was largely preserved over the duration of the experi-ment. The notable exception is rat #2, whose classification image was initially poorly defined. This can be taken as an indication that, at the onset of the test with the random tripods, the animal did not consistently apply a unique strategy to process these stimuli. The strategy, however, stabilized later, as indicated by the cleaner classification images obtained from the second set of trials onward.

(B) To quantify the stability of a rat’s perceptual strategy over time, we measured how well each of

the classification images computed over the four consecutive sets of trials (i.e., the classification images shown in A) predicted rat responses to a test set of held-out stimuli (once plugged into the logistic regression model of eq. 1; see STAR Methods for details). As in Figure 4B, the goodness of the prediction was assessed by computing the logloss cost function, which was remarkably stable over time for all the rats, again with the exception of rat #2 (dots show means over 10 runs of the analysis ± SEM; see STAR Methods for details).

(C) As a final test for the stability of rat perceptual strategies, we computed the similarity between the

predicted perceptual discriminabilities of the 11 distractors according to each of the classification images shown in A. That is, we obtained a matrix of Euclidean distances between pairs of predicted discriminability vectors, similar to the one shown in Figure 3B (left), only that now each rat contribut-ed four classification images (one for each consecutive set of trials), thus resulting in a 24x24 matrix. We then averaged the elements of the matrix above the diagonal, after dividing them in two groups: one including only within-rat comparisons (i.e., 6 comparisons per rat, for a total of 36 values) and the other including only between-rat comparisons (240 values). The bar plot shows the resulting average Euclidean distances obtained for the two groups (means ± SEM). Critically, the within-rat distance was significantly lower than the between-rat distance (p < 0.05; one-tailed t-test), thus showing that, on average, the classification images obtained for the same rat across time were more similar to each other than to the classification images of the other animals. This further demonstrates the stability over time of rat perceptual strategies.

(8)

Rat #1 Rat #2 Rat #3 Rat #4 Rat #5 Rat #6 Sessions in which the regular-size (30º), full-body random tripods were tested

Regular Stimuli 14,477 13,901 12,482 15,919 14,431 15,257 Random Tripod Stimuli 1,809 1,354 1,571 2,112 1,999 2,433

Sessions in which the regular-size (30º), outline random tripods were tested

Regular Stimuli 15,569 14,042 17,652 15,796 15,541 15,715 Random Tripod Stimuli 2,757 1,499 3,059 2,819 2,901 3,08

Sessions in which the small-size (25º), full-body random tripods were tested

Regular Stimuli 22,175 20,579 14,516 21,418 19,425 21,189 Random Tripod Stimuli 2,705 1,867 2,076 2,878 2,823 3,138

Table S1. Number of trials collected in the three phase of the experiment where the

random tripods were presented along with the regular stimuli (i.e., the reference

and distractor objects), Related to Figure 2 and STAR Methods.

   

Riferimenti

Documenti correlati

As a consequence, the implementation of tuition fees also reinforces institutional autonomy, while the implementation of equality of opportunities funding incentives allows a

Search for the isotropic stochastic background using data from Advanced LIGO ’s second observing runB. Abbott

In this section the results of the seismic vulnerability assessment of the triumphal arch carried out by the authors are presented, aimed not only at retracing the

We also compared the convergence power spectrum measured directly from the lensing simulation maps with predictions obtained by analytically integrating the non-linear 3D matter

One can easily verify that every element of ~ is a right tail of 5 preordered withrespect to relation defined 1n (j).. A CHARACTERIZATION DF V-PRIME ANO 5TRONGLY V-PRIME ELHlrtHS OF

Dixit, who performed structural damage detection, which required accurate analysis compared results based on Timoshenko element of a damaged single beam with that of

In this section we recall some features of our model, including a basic renewal theory representation, originally proven in [3], and we derive some new

The paper summarise the results of a large verification experiment, in which the numerical previsions obtained through a new Pyramid Tracing program are compared with