• Non ci sono risultati.

Characterizing axonal myelination within the healthy population: a tract-by-tract mapping of effects of age and gender on the fiber g-ratio

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Condividi "Characterizing axonal myelination within the healthy population: a tract-by-tract mapping of effects of age and gender on the fiber g-ratio"

Copied!
10
0
0

Testo completo

(1)

Characterizing axonal myelination within the healthy population:

a tract-by-tract mapping of effects of age and gender on the

fiber

g-ratio

Mara Cercignani

a,b,*

, Giovanni Giulietti

b

, Nick G. Dowell

a

, Matt Gabel

a

, Rebecca Broad

b

,

P. Nigel Leigh

b

, Neil A. Harrison

a

, Marco Bozzali

b

aDepartment of Neuroscience, Clinical Imaging Sciences Centre, Brighton and Sussex Medical School, University of Sussex, Brighton, East Sussex, UK bNeuroimaging Laboratory, Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy

a r t i c l e i n f o

Article history: Received 5 May 2016

Received in revised form 22 September 2016 Accepted 22 September 2016

Available online 30 September 2016 Keywords: Myelination White matter Axonal conduction Lateralization

a b s t r a c t

The g-ratio, equal to the ratio of the inner-to-outer diameter of a myelinated axon, is associated with the speed of conduction, and thus reflects axonal function and integrity. It is now possible to estimate an “aggregate” g-ratio in vivo using MRI. The aim of this study was to assess the variation of the MRI-derived fiber g-ratio in the brain of healthy individuals, and to characterize its variation across the lifespan. Thirty-eight healthy participants, aged between 20 and 76, were recruited. Whole-brain g-ratio maps were computed and analyzed voxel-wise. Median tract g-ratio values were also extracted. No significant effect of gender was found, whereas age was found to be significantly associated with the g-ratio within the white matter. The tract-specific analysis showed this relationship to follow a nearly-linear increase, although the slope appears to slow down slightly after the 6th decade of life. The most likely inter-pretation is a subtle but consistent reduction in myelin throughout adulthood, with the density of axons beginning to decrease between the 4th and 5th decade.

Ó 2016 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).

1. Introduction

Myelin is a lipid-protein structure that wraps around axons, acting as an electrical insulator to increase the speed of action potential transmission. The amount of myelin wrapped around a nerve fiber determines its maximum conduction velocity. Initial observations in the peripheral nervous system (PNS), indicated that the myelin sheath thickness scales linearly with axon diameter (Berthold et al., 1983), prompting the concept of the“g-ratio”, equal to the ratio of the inner (axon only) to outer diameter (axon þ myelin) of a nervefiber. Models of axonal conductivity propose (Rushton, 1934) that the optimal g-ratio (maximizing fiber con-duction speed) in the PNS is around 0.6. However, values measured in the central nervous system (CNS) have been consistently higher (Waxman and Bennett, 1972), necessitating a revision of Rushton’s model of the optimal g-ratio to include additional factors that might influence its value in the CNS. When the simultaneous aims

of reducing conduction delays, promoting conduction fidelity, lowering energy costs, and space saving are factored into this revised model, the theoretically optimal CNS g-ratio is calculated to be around 0.7 (Chomiak and Hu, 2009).

The tight empirical relationship between g-ratio and conduction speed suggests that g-ratio may represent a fundamental rela-tionship between axonal anatomy and function. Region-specific changes in g-ratio have been described during neurodevelopment (Berthold et al., 1983) and may also provide a sensitive marker of pathology in a number of neurological conditions (Albert et al., 2007). The g-ratio is expected to relate the complex interaction of demyelination, remyelination, and axonal degeneration with neurophysiological abnormalities and thus with clinical symptoms. In particular, as the g-ratio can modify neuronal conduction velocity, it is conceivable that even subtle changes in g-ratio, likely to occur with aging, may mediate age-related cognitive decline.

Imaging (Bartzokis et al., 2012; Sullivan and Pfefferbaum, 2006; Yeatman et al., 2014) and histological (Almeida and Lyons, 2014; Nave and Werner, 2014) studies have confirmed that myelination changes throughout the lifespan. Myelin content increases in the first 2 or 3 decades of life, before reaching a plateau, then gradually reducing in the 5th to 8th decades (Yeatman et al., 2014).

* Corresponding author at: Clinical Imaging Sciences Centre, University of Sussex, Falmer, Brighton, East Sussex BN1 9RR, UK. Tel.:þ44 1273 677879; fax: þ44 1273 876721.

E-mail address:m.cercignani@bsms.ac.uk(M. Cercignani).

Contents lists available atScienceDirect

Neurobiology of Aging

j o u r n a l h o me p a g e : w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o ca t e / n e u a g i n g

0197-4580/Ó 2016 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).

(2)

Interestingly, this degeneration does not occur uniformly across the brain but is typically faster for association tracts and more anterior white matter structures (Bartzokis et al., 2012; Pfefferbaum et al., 2000). Although an increase in the g-ratio is expected to parallel myelin reduction, the processes that regulate myelination in the adult brain are far from trivial (Nave and Werner, 2014), and direct investigations of g-ratio changes within the CNS have, to date, been difficult to implement.

Techniques to measure g-ratio in vivo have only recently become available. Thefirst example was presented byStikov et al. (2011), who combined fractional anisotropy (FA) from diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and quantitative magnetization transfer (MT) to compute a mean voxel g-ratio in the corpus callosum. The same group later proposed a refined method (Stikov et al., 2015a), based on more sophisticated models of diffusion MRI (dMRI) than DTI, which can estimate a whole-brain “aggregate” g-ratio within a voxel. The term“aggregate” is used to indicate that the model as-sumes the g-ratio of all axons in an imaging voxel to be the same, and the resulting value is therefore a summary measure that does not provide any detail of the distribution of g-ratios within the voxel. The proposed model builds on simple geometric consider-ations and exploits the respective sensitivities of MT imaging to myelin, and of dMRI to intra-axonal water fraction. This method was validated against histology in the corpus callosum of the ma-caque brain (Stikov et al., 2015b), and further revised byWest et al. (2016), who showed, using both theoretical considerations and histological validation, that the voxel-wise g-ratio estimated by MRI is equal to the root-mean-square of the g-ratios of individual axons weighted by their area. In other words, larger axons (which also tend to have higher g-ratios) will contribute more to the estimated mean g-ratio value than smaller axons.

This conceptual advance has opened the opportunity to use MRI to characterize whole-brain g-ratio in vivo. This is of particular importance since myelination varies considerably across tracts and varies markedly even within fibers belonging to the same tract (Tomassy et al., 2014) despite the optimal g-ratio expected in the CNS. This variability was confirmed by a recent paper (Mohammadi et al., 2015) providing thefirst g-ratio values estimated by MRI in 6

different white matter tracts from 37 young healthy participants. The results of this study, together with data from electron micro-scopy (Tomassy et al., 2014) suggest a poorly understood mecha-nism for the spatially-specific regulation of the myelination process. Furthermore, myelination of specific fibers has been shown to vary in a functionally-dependent manner (Scholz et al., 2009; Wang and Young, 2014).

The aim of this paper is to explore the anatomical distribution and the interhemispheric differences of the g-ratio in the healthy human brain and to investigate the dependence of the myelin volume fraction (MVF) and the axon volume fraction (AVF) on age and gender. In this way, we provide an interpretation of how myelin thickness andfiber density separately contribute to the observed variations in g-ratio mapping throughout the lifespan.

2. Methods 2.1. Participants

Thirty-eight right-handed healthy volunteers (18 males, median age: 45.5 years, range: 20e76 years) took part in the study after giving written informed consent. Participants were recruited through classified advertisements and university mailing lists. For a detailed distribution of age and gender, seeFig. 1. All participants were screened to exclude any neurological or psychiatric condition. The data were acquired as part of 2 studies approved, respectively, by the Herefordshire and by London and South East National Research Ethics Committees.

2.2. Magnetic resonance imaging

The 2 studies were run in parallel and MRI data were acquired on the same scanner, operating at 1.5 T (Siemens Magnetom Avanto, Erlangen, Germany), with a maximum gradient strength of 44mTm1. The manufacture’s 32-channel head coil was used for signal reception, whereas the body coil was used for transmission. A high-resolution T1-weighted volume (MPRAGE) was acquired for every participant.

(3)

Multi-shell diffusion-weighted (DW) MRI was acquired with single-shot, twice-refocused pulse-gradient spin-echo EPI (Reese et al., 2003), using 3 b values (9 directions with b¼ 300 smm2,

30 directions with b¼ 800 smm2, and 60 diffusion directions with

b ¼ 2400 smm2), optimized for neurite orientation dispersion and density imaging (NODDI, [Zhang et al., 2012]). Ten non-diffusioneweighted (b ¼ 0) volumes were also acquired. The remaining parameters were as follows: TE¼ 99 ms, TR ¼ 8400 ms, matrix¼ 96  96, FoV ¼ 240  240 mm2, slice thickness¼ 2.5 mm.

The total scan time for the NODDI protocol was approximately 16 minutes.

Quantitative MT imaging was based on balanced steady-state free precession (bSSFP) model of MT, introduced byGloor et al. (2008). The acquisition sequence consisted of a 3D True Fast Imaging with Steady-state Precession (TrueFisp) sequence (field of view¼ 240 mm  180 mm, matrix ¼ 256  96, slices ¼ 32, slice thickness¼ 5 mm), modified to allow the duration of the radio-frequency pulse to be varied. Twenty-four volumes were acquired varying either theflip angle (between 5and 40) or the repetition

time (between 3.66 ms and 5.96 ms) and the pulse duration. In addition, 3 three-dimensional fast low-angle shot (FLASH) volumes were acquired for T1-mapping, with repetition time¼ 30 ms and echo time¼ 5 ms. The excitation flip angles were varied between volumes (5, 15, 25). The samefield of view, matrix, and number of slices as the TrueFISP were used. The total scan time for the MT and T1 mapping data wasw8 minutes.

2.3. Computation of parametric maps

DW MRI data were corrected for eddy current effects and involuntary movement by affine coregistration using the FLIRT tool (part of the FMRIB Software Library; FSL [Jenkinson et al., 2002]). Briefly, a within-b-value coregistration was performed, and average b¼ 0, b ¼ 300, b ¼ 800, and b ¼ 2400 images were created. Each of these averages was coregistered to the mean b¼ 0 image to obtain the transformation matching them. Each DW volume was realigned to the mean DW image with the same b-value and the trans-formation matching each DW volume with the b¼ 0 image was obtained by combining the partial transformations from either previous steps. The b matrices were rotated accordingly (Leemans and Jones, 2009).

NODDI is an advanced model of diffusion which enables the estimation of 3 separate water compartments: intracellular, extra-cellular, and isotropic (i.e., CSF). The NODDI toolbox (http://www. nitrc.org/projects/noddi_toolbox) was used to fit the NODDI model to the movement-corrected data and to estimate the following metrics: neurite density index, corresponding to the intracellular volume fraction (vic), orientation dispersion index, and

isotropic volume fraction (viso). The diffusion tensor and FA map

were also estimated using the ordinary least-squares fitting in Camino (http://camino.cs.ucl.ac.uk/;Cook et al., 2006).

MT-bSSFP and T1 mapping volumes were spatially realigned to the 25flip angle FLASH volume using rigid-body registration (FSLflirt, [Jenkinson et al., 2002]). T1 maps were obtained byfitting the 3 FLASH volumes to theoretical voxel values for the spoiled gradient echo for the 3flip angles (Venkatesan et al., 1998).

The MT parameters forward exchange rate constant (kf), T2of

free water component (T2f), and bound proton fraction (F) were

then obtained by performing a voxel-wise nonlinear least-squares fitting (Levenberg-Marquardt method) to a binary spin bath model (Gloor et al., 2008), using in-house software written in C.

To compensate for the spatial distortions in the DW EPI images, nonlinear warping implemented in Advanced Normalization Tools (ANTs;http://stnava.github.io/ANTs) was used to match the FA map to the 25flip angle FLASH volume, to compute the transformation

from DW-MRI to MT space. The same transformation was applied to the vicand visomap.

2.4. Computation of g-ratio

G-ratio maps were obtained as described byStikov et al. (2015b). This model assumes the g-ratio to be constant within a voxel, which leads to the equivalence:

g ¼ ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi 1MVFFVF r ¼ ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi  1þMVFAVF 1 s ; [1]

where g is the g-ratio, MVF is the myelin volume fraction, FVF is the fiber volume fraction, and AVF is the axon volume fraction.

Following Stikov et al. (Stikov et al., 2015b), we set

MVF ¼ kF [2]

and

AVF ¼ ð1  MVFÞð1  visoÞvic; [3]

where k is proportionality constant. 2.5. Estimation of proportionality constant k

The proportionality constant k in Eq[2]is not known a priori. It can vary depending on the specific MT method or model used (Stikov et al., 2015b) and requires histological validation. Previous attempts to estimate its value have led to inconsistent results (Dula et al., 2010; Thiessen et al., 2013).

Here we used a simple approach to derive k from a subsample of our data (Cercignani et al., 2016), similar to that used by Mohammadi et al. (2015). As the variation of g with age is not known a priori, we used only data from participants younger than 30 years of age (N¼ 17; M/F ¼ 7/10; mean age ¼ 25.7, standard deviation [SD]¼ 6.7 years).

Each participant’s FA map (already coregistered with MT data) was nonlinearly coregistered (ANTs 1.9x) to the 2-mm3 isotropic resolution JHU FA template (Hua et al., 2008), available with FSL (http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/Atlases). The same warps were applied to F, vicand visomaps. Afigure demonstrating the goodness

of registration achieved following this pipeline can be found as Supplementary Fig. 1. The JHU white matter tractography atlas was used to extract unbiased masks of the forceps major (thresholded at 20%). The mean F, vicand visoof this tract were estimated for every

participant and then used to estimate the g-ratio for scaling factors k ranging from 1 to 5 and averaged across participant. The resulting g-ratio values were plotted against k to identify the study-specific value corresponding to g-ratioz 0.7.

2.6. Standard space and tract-specific g-ratio

Once k has been estimated, we can compute Eqs[2] and [3] voxel-wise, for each subject, using the coregistered vic, viso, and F maps.

To warp the g-ratio maps into standard Montreal Neurological Institute space for voxel-wise analysis, the same procedure described in the section“estimation of proportionality constant k” was followed for all participants.

The MPRAGE was segmented into white matter, gray matter, and CSF using the VBM8 pipeline (http://www.neuro.uni-jena.de/vbm/), and the white matter segment was thresholded at p > 0.5 and binarised. The resulting white matter masks were coregistered with the g-ratio maps in native space.

Finally, to obtain tract-specific median g-ratio values, 20 white matter tract masks were obtained from the JHU white matter

(4)

tractography atlas (thresholded at 20% and binarized). The tracts contained in the atlas are: right and left anterior thalamic radiation, right and left cortico-spinal tract, forceps minor, forceps major, right and left cingulum bundles; right and left hippocampal portions of the cingulum; right and left inferior fronto-occipital fasciculi; right and left inferior longitudinal fasciculi; right and left superior longi-tudinal fasciculi; right and left temporal portion of the superior longitudinal fasciculi; left and right uncinate fasciculi. Using the in-verse transformation obtained from the warping, the masks were transformed into each participant native space (MT space), combined with each participant’s white matter mask (to remove any voxel falling outside of this tissue because of atrophy) and overlaid onto the coregistered g-ratio maps to extract the median values.

2.7. Statistical analysis

A voxel-wise analysis of g-ratio variation with age and gender was performed using a general linear model and estimating the p-values with permutation tests (with the FSL tool randomize [Winkler et al., 2014]). Two factors were modeled: gender and age. Initially, we also modeled the gender-by-age interaction. The analysis was restricted within the white matter, defined using the SPM8 white matter probability mask, thresholded at 0.6 and binarised. We used 2000 iterations, and the threshold-free-cluster-enhancement (TFCE [Smith and Nichols, 2009],) correction to account for multiple comparisons. Corrected p-values of less than 0.05 were accepted as statistically significant.

All the analyses of tract-specific g-ratio values were performed in SPSS (IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 22.0. Armonk, NY, USA: IBM Corp). For each tract, an independent sample t-test was used to assess the effect of gender; the Pearson’s correlation coef-ficient was used to assess the presence of associations between the g-ratio and age; whereas a paired-sample t-test was used to assess the interhemispheric difference. Bonferroni correction was used, and therefore we accepted as significant p-values <0.0025 for the

independent sample t-tests and the correlations, and p< 0.005 for the paired-sample t-test.

To gain better insight into the variations occurring in g-ratio with age, we plotted the median values against age for each tract. To interpret what drives the variation in g-ratio throughout the lifespan, we also plotted the median tract values of both MVF and AVF against age. For each quantity, we compared 1st and 2nd order polynomial fits to assess which one better describes the data for each tract using the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC, [Schwartz, 1978]), which chooses the best and most economical analytic model.

3. Results

All images were reviewed by an experienced observer to exclude the presence of any macroscopic abnormalities, and to qualitatively assess the raw data. In particular, MT-bSSFP images were inspected to evaluate the amount of banding artifacts; these were considered acceptable when restricted to the edges of the temporal lobe (which is the typical occurrence at 1.5 T). DW-MRI data were reviewed to exclude the presence of artifacts other than susceptibility-induced geometric distortions. None of the data sets were excluded based on these criteria. Among the older participants, 4 (2 males and 2 females, all older than 65 years) showed signs of minor small vessel disease, consistent with age.

3.1. Estimation of proportionality constant k

Fig. 2shows the simulated g-ratio values for the forceps major. These data suggest that a value of k¼ 2.5 provides a g-ratio of w0.7 for our data. This value was used for all the subsequent calculations based on Eq[2].

3.2. Voxel-wise analysis

Fig. 3shows maps of the mean, SD, and coefficient of variation (SD/mean%) of the g-ratio across all 38 participants. These data

Fig. 2. G-ratio estimated from the mean F, vic, and visoof the forceps major. Values are averaged across subjects and plotted against k. The bars indicate the cross-subject standard

(5)

confirm that the estimated g-ratio in white matter lies predomi-nantly between 0.6 and 0.8, with 0.7 being the most represented value. The very low CoV (<10% in most of the white matter) also indicated good reproducibility and excellent between-subject alignment (within the white matter).

No significant age-by-gender interaction was found anywhere in the brain, therefore we removed the interaction term from the model to assess between-gender differences and the association with age.

No significant effect of gender was found. By contrast, age was found to be significantly associated with the g-ratio almost every-where within the white matter (Fig. 4).

3.3. Tract-specific analysis

The mean, standard deviation, and range of tract volume after warping into native space and combination with white matter masks are reported inTable 1.

Consistent with the voxel-wise analysis, no gender effect was observed for the g-ratio in any tract, with the exception of the left ILF, where females had higher mean g-ratio (0.711) than males (0.694), with p¼ 0.002.Fig. 5shows the median g-ratio per tract, separately for 3 age groups: 20e40 (younger), 41e60 (middle-aged), and over 60 (older). For the purpose of thisfigure data from the left and right hemisphere were pooled. No tract showed a median ratio lower than 0.67. The younger group had a lower g-ratio than the other 2 groups in all tracts, with the lowest values found in the forceps minor (0.671), and the largest in the cingulum bundle (0.758). Differences between the middle-aged and the older groups were less pronounced and less systematic than those observed between the younger and the middle-aged groups.

G-ratio was significantly correlated with age for each of the tracts studied (p < 0.05), with the exception of the right cingulum (p¼ 0.1). These correlations remained significant after Bonferroni correction (p < 0.0025) with the exception of the right inferior longitudinal fasciculus, the uncinate fasciculus

(bilaterally), and the hippocampal portion of the cingulum bundle (bilaterally).

When plotting median g-ratio against age, some tracts showed a linear increase with age (Fig. 6, e.g., the cortico-spinal tract and the SLF). Others showed a more quadratic dependency with the aggregate g-ratio increasing between 20 and 60 years of age then reaching a plateau (Fig. 6, e.g., IFOF). Finally, for some tracts, such as the cingulum bundle, no obvious dependency could be observed. Nevertheless, the results of the BIC comparison indicate that the linear and quadratic models perform equally in most of the tracts. Significant (p < 0.05) inter-hemisphere differences were observed for most tracts with the exception of the cingulum bundle, the hippocampal part of the cingulum bundle, the anterior thalamic radiation, and the inferior longitudinal fasciculus. The laterality effect remained significant (p < 0.005) after Bonferroni correction for the uncinate fasciculus, the SLF, the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus, and the cortico-spinal tract, with higher values in the right compared to the left (dominant) hemisphere. Laterality seemed to be maintained throughout the life span (see ATR and uncinate fasciculus inFig. 6).

When we looked at the variability with age of MVF and AVF, we observed that MVF tends to decrease with age in most tracts (see Fig. 7, top panel). The variations in AVF were more tract-specific. In some tracts, AVF appeared relatively stable across the lifespan (e.g., Forceps major and forceps minor). However, in other tracts it appeared to increase to approximately 50 years of age then decrease (e.g., ATR, IFOF, ILF, and uncinate fasciculus). In a few other tracts, the increase in AVF appeared to remain constant until very late in life (e.g., CST, SLF, cingulate bundle).Fig. 7shows 3 tracts as representative examples. Again, we compared linear and 2nd-order polynomialfits using the BIC, which indicates that the linear model is to be preferred for MVF in all tracts. Conversely, for AVF, the quadratic model performs better in the anterior thalamic radia-tions, the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus, the inferior longitu-dinal fasciculus, the SLF, and the forceps minor.

Fig. 3. Voxel-wise maps of the mean (top), standard deviation (middle), and coefficient of variation (CoV, bottom) for the g-ratio computed across participants. The mean value in the white matter is in agreement with the expected value of 0.7. The inter-subject CoV is very low in the white matter (<8%).

(6)

4. Discussion

Here we present a detailed investigation of the factors affecting the variability of the g-ratio within the CNS. The mainfindings can be summarized as follows: gender has no influence on regional g-ratio; age significantly modulates the g-ratio; and there is evidence for g-ratio laterality in some association tracts. In addition, we showed that substantial g-ratio variability exists within the brain. 4.1. G-ratio dependency on gender

The lack of evidence for a gender-dependent g-ratio difference might be surprising, in light of the previously-reported sex differ-ences in the volume and microstructure of the white matter (Good

et al., 2001; Hsu et al., 2008). Gender differences within the CNS are known to occur during neurodevelopment (Perrin et al., 2009), and a between-gender shift in the g-ratio has previously been proposed as a possible explanation for the emergence of sex differences during adolescence (Paus and Toro, 2009). Our data, by contrast, indicate that g-ratio is not significantly influenced by sex, at least not in adults. This result should be interpreted with caution, as the sample size was relatively small, and, admittedly, male participants were on average slightly older than females in our sample, but this difference was not statistically significant (independent sample t-test, p¼ 0.13). Furthermore, when the gender-by-age interaction was incorporated into the model it did not change ourfindings. We speculate that the absence of a gender effect on g-ratio does not imply that such an effect does not exist for the absolute degree of myelination, but that the relationship between inner and outer axonal diameters is not gender dependent. This is conceivable in light of its functional interpretation, that is, being a determinant of speed of conduction.

Table 1

Volume [mm3] of white matter tracts

Tract Side Mean SD Range

ATR R 6372.302 1059.814 4139.648 9654.785 L 5526.817 1148.663 3603.516 8222.168 CST R 5857.101 662.819 4407.715 7092.773 L 5291.247 592.1852 4091.309 6697.266 Cingulum R 1505.011 357.8177 224.1211 2078.613 L 539.3709 203.9112 153.8086 927.2461 Cingulum HP R 237.6516 82.47427 215.332 435.0586 L 394.5826 119.1907 101.0742 628.418 Forceps major 6358.655 1186.431 3331.055 8674.805 Forceps minor 18,810.33 2568.761 13,249.51 25,659.67 IFOF R 7241.147 1120.903 5453.613 10,331.54 L 7797.055 1261.921 5124.023 11,025.88 ILF R 6721.898 1000.217 4473.633 8767.09 L 4655.543 810.1528 2997.07 6626.953 SLF R 8935.585 1137.081 6886.23 11,153.32 L 7309.493 891.228 5172.363 9017.578 SLF HP R 1388.325 290.4695 716.3086 2056.641 L 679.9959 180.6508 193.3594 1107.422 Key: ATR, anterior, thalamic radiation; CST, cortico-spinal tract; HP, hippocampal portion; IFOF, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus; ILF, inferior longitudinal fascic-ulus; L, left; R, right; SD, standard deviation; SLF, superior longitudinal fasciculus.

Fig. 5. Comparison of mean g-ratio across all the white matter tracts studied, for participants grouped in 3 age groups. Values of left and right were pooled for bilateral tracts. Abbreviations: ATR, anterior, thalamic radiation; CST, cortico-spinal tract; ILF, inferior longitudinal fasciculus; SLF, superior longitudinal fasciculus.

Fig. 4. Results of the voxel-wise correlation between g-ratio and age. Voxels in red indicate positive association, with p< 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons using the TFCE method. The analysis was restricted to the white matter. Abbreviation: TFCE, threshold free cluster enhancement. (For interpretation of the references to color in thisfigure legend, the reader is referred to the Web version of this article.)

(7)

4.2. G-ratio dependency on age

By contrast, age has a very significant effect on the g-ratio: this was evident from both voxel-wise and the tract-specific analysis. This result is in keeping with previous reports indicating changes in the white matter with aging (Sullivan and Pfefferbaum, 2006) and indicates that the g-ratio increases with age, consistent with the expected thinning of the myelin sheath (Peters, 2009). While specific changes in the g-ratio during the lifespan have never been investi-gated in the human brain, histological data from the sciatic nerve (Ugrenovic et al., 2016) indicate that in the PNS the percentage of myelinated nervefibers with large diameter and optimal g-ratio declines significantly with age. As demonstrated by West et al. (2016), MRI provides a g-ratio estimate that is an average across the whole voxel, weighted by the axonal area. Consequently, changes to this quantity could reflect not only global changes in the inner-to-outer axonal diameter ratio but also a change in the distribution of g-ratios within the voxel, and the balance between small- and large-diameter axons. Fibers with smaller diameters tend to have slightly lower g-ratios (Berthold et al., 1983), and therefore a selective loss of large-diameter axons would lead to an overall reduced voxel-averaged g-ratio. Based on the data we have, however, we can only conclude that the aggregate g-ratio tends to increase with age throughout the brain, and histological validation would be required to provide a definite interpretation of these findings.

Nevertheless, further information can be gathered from the analysis of MVF and AVF, and of their variability with age. We observed almost monotonic decreases in MVF, although the trend was different for different tracts. The BIC comparison confirmed that a 1st-order model better describes MVF variability with age than a 2nd-order one. Thisfinding is in contrast with the obser-vations ofYeatman et al. (2014), who used R1 (¼ 1/T1) as a proxy for myelination in a group 102 participants, aged 7 to 85. They found an

inverted U-shaped trend which was symmetric around the peak. It is unclear why we were not able to reproduce their findings; however, it is possible that, not having data for subjects younger than 20, given the variance around the mean, and the relatively small sample size, we are simply unable to capture the initial in-crease in MVF. Interestingly, AVF seems to vary with age in a nonlinear fashion (at least for some tracts). As expected, AVF tends to decrease after the age of 50 for most tracts, but before that age we observed an apparent increase. Previous studies based on im-aging have shown nonlinear changes in diffusivities, fractional anisotropy, and white matter volume (Walhovd et al., 2014). While a direct comparison between those data and AVF is not possible, evidence from volumetric studies suggests that white matter vol-ume as a whole peaks around 40 years of age (Westlye et al., 2010). We could therefore conclude that the observed variation in AVF results from the complex sequence of maturation and aging pro-cesses in the white matter.

White matter changes with aging have been amply demon-strated using a range of imaging techniques, including DTI and relaxometry (Billiet et al., 2015), all supporting the hypothesis that some white matter structures are more vulnerable than others to the aging process. One of the theories developed to explain this phenomenon is the so-called retrogenesis hypothesis (Brickman et al., 2012), which postulates that late-myelinated white matter fibers are most vulnerable to age- and disease-related degenera-tion. While our data suggest greater myelin density in the CST and corpus callosum as opposed to the UF and ILF, they do not support the hypothesis that the loss of myelin in the latter tracts is faster or onsets earlier than in the former ones. We are not thefirst to fail to confirm this theory, with some previous studies even suggesting an increase of myelination with age (Billiet et al., 2015).

While no previous study investigated g-ratio variations with age in adults, 2 previous reports looked, respectively, at changes in

Fig. 6. Scatterplots of median tract g-ratio versus age for 6 representative tracts out of the 10 reconstructed. Data for the right (gray squares) and left (black diamonds) hemisphere are shown, with the exception of the forceps, where gray square represent data from the forceps minor and black diamonds represent data from the forceps major. Abbreviations: ATR, anterior thalamic radiation; CST, cortico-spinal tract; IFOF, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus; SLF, superior longitudinal fasciculus.

(8)

infants (aged 3 months to 7 years;Dean et al., 2016) and preterm babies (Melbourne et al., 2014). As expected, the g-ratio was found to be approximately 1 (no myelin) at very early ages, sharply decreasing during the first 2 years of life, to follow a slower decrease later on toward an asymptotic value of 0.7e0.8. Interest-ingly, Dean et al. (2016)found the asymptotic value to differ for distinct white matter regions, with the cingulum, the SLF, and the internal capsules reaching higher values than the corpus callosum. This is in line with ourfindings of higher g-ratio in young adults in the cingulum and in the SLF, compared to the forceps.

4.3. Anatomical distribution of g-ratio

Despite the relatively homogeneous rate-of-variation of the g-ratio along the lifespan, our data confirm a substantial anatomical variability of this parameter (see Fig. 5), even in young participants. Thisfits with the preliminary observations by Mohammadi et al. (2015). When comparing our tract-specific estimations of the g-ratio with theirs, however, some important differences appear. They found the cingulum bundle, the optic radiation, and the IFOF to have significantly lower g-ratios than other tracts such as the CST, the fornix and the optic radiation. While we looked at a slightly different sample of tracts, we found the cingulum to have the highest g-ratio, in clear contradiction with the previous report. We can only speculate of what might be the origin of this disparity, probably related to methodological considerations. First, we used a different white matter tract atlas, and different thresholds (we retained all voxels with a probability of greater than 20% of being part of the tract, while they only used those with probability higher than 50%). When computing the mean/median over the whole tract, it is possible that voxels in the “periphery” (which are likely to have been included in larger numbers here than in the previous study) contribute

higher g-ratio values than the ones in the core. Another, perhaps more important, observation is that we used Stikov’s original model of the g-ratio (Stikov et al., 2015a), while Mohammadi et al. introduced an alternative method, where thefiber volume density is estimated based on an index called the tensor fiber density (Reisert et al., 2013). While this latter method has the advantage that it can be derived from single shell dMRI data, it is also known to show very high-spatial variability (Reisert et al., 2013). In addition, because of the low b-value used for their acquisition (b ¼ 1000 mm2s1) the tensorfiber density can be affected by contributions from the extracellular water (Mohammadi et al., 2015). Furthermore, the myelin signal contributing to dMRI is negligible even in the presence of sig-nificant myelin volume due to the long echo times associated with dMRI and the comparatively-short T2 of myelin. Impor-tantly, Stikov’s model explicitly takes this into account and accordingly corrects the AVF, and thus the FVF, whereas the tensor density approach does not. All these factors are likely to affect thefinal g-ratio estimation, and only detailed validations in animal models will be able to conclusively determine the accu-racy of either method.

4.4. Estimation of the proportionality constant

Another important limitation, which is common to both methods is the need for the estimation of an arbitrary propor-tionality constant (k in our case), which could potentially introduce some bias. We followed an approach similar to that ofMohammadi et al. (2015), and derived k for thefibers originating from the splenium of the corpus callosum (forceps major), where the g-ratio is expected to be approximately equal to 0.7 (Stikov et al., 2015b). Despite this procedure may be seen as arbitrary, the value we estimated for k of 2.5 is consistent with the value originally derived

Fig. 7. Scatterplots of median tract myelin volume fraction (MVF, top) and axon volume fraction (AVF, bottom) versus age for 3 representative tracts out of the 10 reconstructed. Data for the right (gray squares) and left (black diamonds) hemisphere are shown, with the exception of the forceps, where gray square represent data from the forceps minor and black diamonds represent data from the forceps major. Abbreviation: CST, cortico-spinal tract.

(9)

byDula et al. (2010)from histological validation in the spinal cord white matter. It is however important to highlight that a wrong calibration could lead to the computed g-ratio having some de-pendency on FVF (Campbell et al., 2016). While the resulting quantity is still weighted by the aggregate g-ratio, caution must be used when interpreting changes to it.

4.5. Laterality of g-ratio

It is important to reiterate that, while the g-ratio is a funda-mental quantity, the MRI-estimated g-ratio is a new concept and warrants further work toward its full validation. It is possible that problems and limitations of this technique will only become evident as we continue to develop its applications. Nevertheless, the noninvasive estimation of this parameter opens exciting new possibilities in establishing structure-function relationships within the CNS.

Indeed, we reported that while interhemispheric differences are negligible for most white matter tracts, some association tracts, namely the uncinate fasciculus, the SLF, the inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus, and the cortico-spinal tract, show a very significant lat-erality effect. Left-to-right asymmetries in the association bundles have been reported previously with other methodologies (e.g., [Takao et al., 2011]), and are believed to be related to specialized functions of the brain, such as language, which are lateralized in humans (Parker et al., 2005). Some authors have even suggested a potential link between the absence of asymmetry and psychosis (Park et al., 2004). Although it is unclear what determines this spatially-specific change in myelin thickness, a fascinating hy-pothesis linking it to function postulates that myelination is at least in part regulated by neuronal activity (Lundgaard et al., 2013; Scholz et al., 2009; Wang and Young, 2014). Interestingly, we observed lower g-ratio values in the left (dominant) hemisphere. It is unfortunately not possible to explore any relationship with handedness in this sample, as all participants were right handed. Assessing the relationship between the estimated g-ratio values and the performance at specific tasks testing lateralized functions could perhaps support this intriguing suggestion; however, we did not collect cognitive data in this sample.

4.6. White matter plasticity

As afinal remark, we observe that if “myelin plasticity” exists (Wang and Young, 2014), it is very likely that each individual’s g-ratio is not only affected by easily accountable factors such as age but also by a collection of personalized factors that depend on their education and lifestyle, cognitive stimulation, and physical expe-riences. Studies in rodents have indicated that physical exercise (Krityakiarana et al., 2010) and socialization (Liu et al., 2012) pro-mote white matter plasticity. In this view, a limitation of our study is the lack of supporting data on an individual’s lifestyle and could justify the large variance we observe around the average trend with age (Fig. 6). Future work should address this interesting topic and establish the relationship between the g-ratio and factors that can promote neural activity and therefore increase myelin production via modulation of oligodendrocyte progenitor cells proliferation (Wang and Young, 2014).

4.7. Methodological considerations

It is important to reiterate that the aggregate g-ratio estimated by MRI relies on a number of assumptions with respect to the 2 underlying techniques, MT and NODDI. Both methods suffer from some limitations and any error to the parametric maps derived from them will of course propagate into the g-ratio. While F has

been shown to correlate with myelination (e.g., [Turati et al., 2015]), MT is also sensitive to other factors, such as edema, inflammation, and pH (Stanisz et al., 2004; Vavasour et al., 2011). NODDI enables the estimation of vicand visousing clinically feasible acquisition

protocols; however, this comes at the price of making some strong assumption about the intracellular diffusivity (Zhang et al., 2012). In addition, the estimation of visocan be biased in the white matter

(Bouyagoub et al., 2016), and such a bias can affect the estimated AVF. With respect to this study in particular, a further limitation is that the resolution of the 2 techniques was not matched, potentially leading to misregistration and partial volume effects.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, we have shown that the g-ratio varies throughout the life span with a tendency to increase. The corresponding vari-ation in AVF and MVF support the hypothesis that a subtle but consistent reduction in myelin occurs throughout adulthood, whereas axon density follows a more complex pattern on initial increase followed by a decrease. As this is a cross-sectional study, any“change” with age is of course only inferred and longitudinal studies are needed to confirm these findings, as well as address the exciting hypotheses generated by these data.

Disclosure statement

The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose. Acknowledgements

This study was funded by the Wellcome Trust (WT) Fellowship grant awarded to NAH (WT093881MA), by the MND Association biomedical research project grant (Leigh/Apr2014/824-791) awar-ded to P. Nigel Leigh, and by the Italian Ministry of Health (project grants RF-2013-02358409 and RF-2010-2311484).

Appendix A. Supplementary data

Supplementary data related to this article can be found athttp:// dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.09.016.

References

Albert, M., Antel, J., Bruck, W., Stadelmann, C., 2007. Extensive cortical remyelination in patients with chronic multiple sclerosis. Brain Pathol. 17, 129e138.

Almeida, R.G., Lyons, D.A., 2014. On the resemblance of synapse formation and CNS myelination. Neuroscience 276, 98e108.

Bartzokis, G., Lu, P.H., Heydari, P., Couvrette, A., Lee, G.J., Kalashyan, G., Freeman, F., Grinstead, J.W., Villablanca, P., Finn, J.P., Mintz, J., Alger, J.R., Altshuler, L.L., 2012. Multimodal magnetic resonance imaging assessment of white matter aging trajectories over the lifespan of healthy individuals. Biol. Psychiatry 72, 1026e1034.

Berthold, C.H., Nilsson, I., Rydmark, M., 1983. Axon diameter and myelin sheath thickness in nervefibres of the ventral spinal root of the seventh lumbar nerve of the adult and developing cat. J. Anat. 136, 483e508.

Billiet, T., Vandenbulcke, M., Madler, B., Peeters, R., Dhollander, T., Zhang, H., Deprez, S., Van den Bergh, B.R., Sunaert, S., Emsell, L., 2015. Age-related microstructural differences quantified using myelin water imaging and advanced diffusion MRI. Neurobiol. Aging 36, 2107e2121.

Bouyagoub, S., Dowell, N.G., Hurley, S.A., Wood, T.C., Cercignani, M., 2016. Over-estimation of CSF Fraction in NODDI: Possible Correction Techniques and the Effect on Neurite Density and Orientation Dispersion Measures. Presented at 24th Annual Meeting of the International Society for Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. Singapore.

Brickman, A.M., Meier, I.B., Korgaonkar, M.S., Provenzano, F.A., Grieve, S.M., Siedlecki, K.L., Wasserman, B.T., Williams, L.M., Zimmerman, M.E., 2012. Testing the white matter retrogenesis hypothesis of cognitive aging. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 1699e1715.

Campbell, J., Leppert, I., Boudreau, M., Narayanan, S., Cohen-Adad, J., Pike, G., Stikov, N., 2016. Mapping the Myelin G-ratio: Promises and Pitfalls. Presented at

(10)

24th Annual Meeting of the International Society for Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. Singapore.

Cercignani, M., Giulietti, G., Dowell, N.G., Spano, B., Harrison, N.A., Bozzali, M., 2016. 24th Annual Meeting of the International Society for Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, Singapore.

Chomiak, T., Hu, B., 2009. What is the optimal value of the g-ratio for myelinated fibers in the rat CNS? A theoretical approach. PLoS One 4, e7754.

Cook, P.A., Bai, Y., Nedjati-Gilani, S., Seunarine, K.K., Hall, G.M.G., Parker, J., Alexander, D.C., 2006. Camino: open-source diffusion-MRI reconstruction and processing. In: 14th Scientific Meeting of the International Society for Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, Seattle, WA, USA.

Dean 3rd, D.C., O’Muircheartaigh, J., Dirks, H., Travers, B.G., Adluru, N., Alexander, A.L., Deoni, S.C., 2016. Mapping an index of the myelin g-ratio in infants using magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 132, 225e237.

Dula, A.N., Gochberg, D.F., Valentine, H.L., Valentine, W.M., Does, M.D., 2010. Mul-tiexponential T2, magnetization transfer, and quantitative histology in white matter tracts of rat spinal cord. Magn. Reson. Med. 63, 902e909.

Gloor, M., Scheffler, K., Bieri, O., 2008. Quantitative magnetization transfer imaging using balanced SSFP. Magn. Reson. Med. 60, 691e700.

Good, C.D., Johnsrude, I., Ashburner, J., Henson, R.N., Friston, K.J., Frackowiak, R.S., 2001. Cerebral asymmetry and the effects of sex and handedness on brain structure: a voxel-based morphometric analysis of 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage 14, 685e700.

Hsu, J.L., Leemans, A., Bai, C.H., Lee, C.H., Tsai, Y.F., Chiu, H.C., Chen, W.H., 2008. Gender differences and age-related white matter changes of the human brain: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Neuroimage 39, 566e577.

Hua, K., Zhang, J., Wakana, S., Jiang, H., Li, X., Reich, D.S., Calabresi, P.A., Pekar, J.J., van Zijl, P.C., Mori, S., 2008. Tract probability maps in stereotaxic spaces: analyses of white matter anatomy and tract-specific quantification. Neuroimage 39, 336e347.

Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M., Smith, S., 2002. Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain im-ages. Neuroimage 17, 825e841.

Krityakiarana, W., Espinosa-Jeffrey, A., Ghiani, C.A., Zhao, P.M., Topaldjikian, N., Gomez-Pinilla, F., Yamaguchi, M., Kotchabhakdi, N., de Vellis, J., 2010. Voluntary exercise increases oligodendrogenesis in spinal cord. Int. J. Neurosci. 120, 280e290.

Leemans, A., Jones, D.K., 2009. The B-matrix must be rotated when correcting for subject motion in DTI data. Magn. Reson. Med. 61, 1336e1349.

Liu, J., Dietz, K., DeLoyht, J.M., Pedre, X., Kelkar, D., Kaur, J., Vialou, V., Lobo, M.K., Dietz, D.M., Nestler, E.J., Dupree, J., Casaccia, P., 2012. Impaired adult myelination in the prefrontal cortex of socially isolated mice. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 1621e1623.

Lundgaard, I., Luzhynskaya, A., Stockley, J.H., Wang, Z., Evans, K.A., Swire, M., Volbracht, K., Gautier, H.O., Franklin, R.J., Ffrench-Constant, Charles, Attwell, D., Káradóttir, R.T., 2013. Neuregulin and BDNF induce a switch to NMDA receptor-dependent myelination by oligodendrocytes. PLoS Biol. 11, e1001743.

Melbourne, A., Eaton-Rosen, Z., De Vita, E., Bainbridge, A., Cardoso, M.J., Price, D., Cady, E., Kendall, G.S., Robertson, N.J., Marlow, N., Ourselin, S., 2014. Multi-modal measurement of the myelin-to-axon diameter g-ratio in preterm-born neonates and adult controls. Med. Image Comput. Comput. Assist. Interv. 17, 268e275.

Mohammadi, S., Carey, D., Dick, F., Diedrichsen, J., Sereno, M.I., Reisert, M., Callaghan, M.F., Weiskopf, N., 2015. Whole-brain in-vivo measurements of the axonal g-ratio in a group of 37 healthy volunteers. Front. Neurosci. 9, 441.

Nave, K.A., Werner, H.B., 2014. Myelination of the nervous system: mechanisms and functions. Annu. Rev. Cell Dev. Biol. 30, 503e533.

Park, H.J., Westin, C.F., Kubicki, M., Maier, S.E., Niznikiewicz, M., Baer, A., Frumin, M., Kikinis, R., Jolesz, F.A., McCarley, R.W., Shenton, M.E., 2004. White matter hemisphere asymmetries in healthy subjects and in schizophrenia: a diffusion tensor MRI study. Neuroimage 23, 213e223.

Parker, G.J., Luzzi, S., Alexander, D.C., Wheeler-Kingshott, C.A., Ciccarelli, O., Lambon Ralph, M.A., 2005. Lateralization of ventral and dorsal auditory-language pathways in the human brain. Neuroimage 24, 656e666.

Paus, T., Toro, R., 2009. Could sex differences in white matter be explained by g ratio? Front. Neuroanat. 3, 14.

Perrin, J.S., Leonard, G., Perron, M., Pike, G.B., Pitiot, A., Richer, L., Veillette, S., Pausova, Z., Paus, T., 2009. Sex differences in the growth of white matter during adolescence. Neuroimage 45, 1055e1066.

Peters, A., 2009. The effects of normal aging on myelinated nervefibers in monkey central nervous system. Front. Neuroanat. 3, 11.

Pfefferbaum, A., Sullivan, E.V., Hedehus, M., Lim, K.O., Adalsteinsson, E., Moseley, M., 2000. Age-related decline in brain white matter anisotropy measured with spatially corrected echo-planar diffusion tensor imaging. Magn. Reson. Med. 44, 259e268.

Reese, T.G., Heid, O., Weisskoff, R.M., Wedeen, V.J., 2003. Reduction of eddy-current-induced distortion in diffusion MRI using a twice-refocused spin echo. Magn. Reson. Med. 49, 177e182.

Reisert, M., Mader, I., Umarova, R., Maier, S., Tebartz van Elst, L., Kiselev, V.G., 2013. Fiber density estimation from single q-shell diffusion imaging by tensor divergence. Neuroimage 77, 166e176.

Rushton, W.A., 1934. A physical analysis of the relation between threshold and interpolar length in the electric excitation of medullated nerve. J. Physiol. 82, 332e352.

Scholz, J., Klein, M.C., Behrens, T.E., Johansen-Berg, H., 2009. Training induces changes in white-matter architecture. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 1370e1371.

Schwartz, G., 1978. Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann. Stat. 6, 461e464.

Smith, S.M., Nichols, T.E., 2009. Threshold-free cluster enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. Neuroimage 44, 83e98.

Stanisz, G.J., Webb, S., Munro, C.A., Pun, T., Midha, R., 2004. MR properties of excised neural tissue following experimentally induced inflammation. Magn. Reson. Med. 51, 473e479.

Stikov, N., Campbell, J.S., Stroh, T., Lavelee, M., Frey, S., Novek, J., Nuara, S., Ho, M.K., Bedell, B.J., Dougherty, R.F., Leppert, I.R., Boudreau, M., Narayanan, S., Duval, T., Cohen-Adad, J., Picard, P.A., Gasecka, A., Côté, D., Pike, G.B., 2015a. In vivo his-tology of the myelin g-ratio with magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 118, 397e405.

Stikov, N., Campbell, J.S., Stroh, T., Lavelee, M., Frey, S., Novek, J., Nuara, S., Ho, M.K., Bedell, B.J., Dougherty, R.F., Leppert, I.R., Boudreau, M., Narayanan, S., Duval, T., Cohen-Adad, J., Picard, P.A., Gasecka, A., Côté, D., Pike, G.B., 2015b. Quantitative analysis of the myelin g-ratio from electron microscopy images of the macaque corpus callosum. Data Brief 4, 368e373.

Stikov, N., Perry, L.M., Mezer, A., Rykhlevskaia, E., Wandell, B.A., Pauly, J.M., Dougherty, R.F., 2011. Bound pool fractions complement diffusion measures to describe white matter micro and macrostructure. Neuroimage 54, 1112e1121.

Sullivan, E.V., Pfefferbaum, A., 2006. Diffusion tensor imaging and aging. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30, 749e761.

Takao, H., Hayashi, N., Ohtomo, K., 2011. White matter asymmetry in healthy in-dividuals: a diffusion tensor imaging study using tract-based spatial statistics. Neuroscience 193, 291e299.

Thiessen, J.D., Zhang, Y., Zhang, H., Wang, L., Buist, R., Del Bigio, M.R., Kong, J., Li, X.M., Martin, M., 2013. Quantitative MRI and ultrastructural examination of the cuprizone mouse model of demyelination. NMR Biomed. 26, 1562e1581.

Tomassy, G.S., Berger, D.R., Chen, H.H., Kasthuri, N., Hayworth, K.J., Vercelli, A., Seung, H.S., Lichtman, J.W., Arlotta, P., 2014. Distinct profiles of myelin distri-bution along single axons of pyramidal neurons in the neocortex. Science 344, 319e324.

Turati, L., Moscatelli, M., Mastropietro, A., Dowell, N.G., Zucca, I., Erbetta, A., Cordiglieri, C., Brenna, G., Bianchi, B., Mantegazza, R., Cercignani, M., Baggi, F., Minati, L., 2015. In vivo quantitative magnetization transfer imaging correlates with histology during de- and remyelination in cuprizone-treated mice. NMR Biomed. 28, 327e337.

Ugrenovic, S., Jovanovic, I., Vasovic, L., Kundalic, B., Cukuranovic, R., Stefanovic, V., 2016. Morphometric analysis of the diameter and g-ratio of the myelinated nervefibers of the human sciatic nerve during the aging process. Anat. Sci. Int. 91, 238e245.

Vavasour, I.M., Laule, C., Li, D.K., Traboulsee, A.L., MacKay, A.L., 2011. Is the magnetization transfer ratio a marker for myelin in multiple sclerosis? J. Magn. Reson. Imaging 33, 713e718.

Venkatesan, R., Lin, W., Haacke, E.M., 1998. Accurate determination of spin-density and T1 in the presence of RF-field inhomogeneities and flip-angle miscalibra-tion. Magn. Reson. Med. 40, 592e602.

Walhovd, K.B., Johansen-Berg, H., Karadottir, R.T., 2014. Unraveling the secrets of white matterdbridging the gap between cellular, animal and human imaging studies. Neuroscience 276, 2e13.

Wang, S., Young, K.M., 2014. White matter plasticity in adulthood. Neuroscience 276, 148e160.

Waxman, S.G., Bennett, M.V., 1972. Relative conduction velocities of small myelin-ated and non-myelinmyelin-atedfibres in the central nervous system. Nat. New Biol. 238, 217e219.

West, K.L., Kelm, N.D., Carson, R.P., Does, M.D., 2016. A revised model for estimating g-ratio from MRI. Neuroimage 125, 1155e1158.

Westlye, L.T., Walhovd, K.B., Dale, A.M., Bjornerud, A., Due-Tonnessen, P., Engvig, A., Grydeland, H., Tamnes, C.K., Ostby, Y., Fjell, A.M., 2010. Life-span changes of the human brain white matter: diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and volumetry. Cereb. Cortex 20, 2055e2068.

Winkler, A.M., Ridgway, G.R., Webster, M.A., Smith, S.M., Nichols, T.E., 2014. Per-mutation inference for the general linear model. Neuroimage 92, 381e397.

Yeatman, J.D., Wandell, B.A., Mezer, A.A., 2014. Lifespan maturation and degenera-tion of human brain white matter. Nat. Commun. 5, 4932.

Zhang, H., Schneider, T., Wheeler-Kingshott, C.A., Alexander, D.C., 2012. NODDI: practical in vivo neurite orientation dispersion and density imaging of the human brain. Neuroimage 61, 1000e1016.

Riferimenti

Documenti correlati

Finally, we used a repeated measures ANOVA (8 time frames: baseline, after one cycle of NIBS, short and long follow-up, after pause, after second cycle of NIBS, second short and

FTIR spectra of sample 2b before reaction (spectrum a), immediately after contact with the mixture of ethylene oxide and carbon dioxide (spectrum b), after 5 hours

But one of the most interesting one is if the old masons were aware about the ancient anti - seismic constructions details or they used those techniques according to other

4 shows the trend as a func- tion of the P/Si ratio at constant water/total solid content (0.47). As expected, the slump values decrease, lowering the amount of water at a fixed

The present study has several clinical implications: (a) to pro- mote mass screening campaigns for the general population in order to identify the presence of subthreshold

It is striking that despite the fact that the vaginal flora has been a topic of interest to physicians and scientists since the last decade of the 19th century, there is still much

This paper shows an EU research on the definition of guidelines for the preparation of an Urban Waste Management Plan; a Plan able to reach an integrated waste system, in

Relativamente alla quantificazione delle micotossine e in particolare dell’OTA, grande importanza hanno i metodi elettrochimici, soprattutto voltammetrici e i biosensori