• Non ci sono risultati.

Blindsight is sensitive to stimulus numerosity and configuration: evidence from the redundant signal effect.

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Condividi "Blindsight is sensitive to stimulus numerosity and configuration: evidence from the redundant signal effect."

Copied!
10
0
0

Testo completo

(1)

24 July 2021

AperTO - Archivio Istituzionale Open Access dell'Università di Torino

Original Citation:

Blindsight is sensitive to stimulus numerosity and configuration: evidence from the redundant signal effect.

Published version:

DOI:10.1007/s00221-015-4236-6

Terms of use:

Open Access

(Article begins on next page)

Anyone can freely access the full text of works made available as "Open Access". Works made available under a Creative Commons license can be used according to the terms and conditions of said license. Use of all other works requires consent of the right holder (author or publisher) if not exempted from copyright protection by the applicable law.

Availability:

This is the author's manuscript

(2)

1 23

Experimental Brain Research ISSN 0014-4819

Exp Brain Res

DOI 10.1007/s00221-015-4236-6

Blindsight is sensitive to stimulus

numerosity and configuration: evidence

from the redundant signal effect

Alessia Celeghin, Silvia Savazzi, Marissa

Barabas, Matteo Bendini & Carlo

(3)

1 23

Your article is protected by copyright and

all rights are held exclusively by

Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg. This e-offprint is

for personal use only and shall not be

self-archived in electronic repositories. If you wish

to self-archive your article, please use the

accepted manuscript version for posting on

your own website. You may further deposit

the accepted manuscript version in any

repository, provided it is only made publicly

available 12 months after official publication

or later and provided acknowledgement is

given to the original source of publication

and a link is inserted to the published article

on Springer's website. The link must be

accompanied by the following text: "The final

publication is available at link.springer.com”.

(4)

1 3

Exp Brain Res

DOI 10.1007/s00221-015-4236-6 RESEARCH ARTICLE

Blindsight is sensitive to stimulus numerosity and configuration:

evidence from the redundant signal effect

Alessia Celeghin · Silvia Savazzi · Marissa Barabas · Matteo Bendini · Carlo A. Marzi

Received: 18 December 2014 / Accepted: 17 February 2015 © Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2015

dots, both of which could be either single or quadruple. The latter could either have a fixed spatial configuration repre-senting a diamond or be randomly spatially assembled on every trial. Both configurations covered the same extent of visual field and had the overall same luminance. We found that a numerosity effect as a result of increasing the num-ber of stimuli in the blind field was indeed present but only with the diamond configuration. This is a convincing evi-dence that this form of blindsight does not depend upon stimulus numerosity per se but is likely to be related to the presence of structured and memorized rather than meaning-less changing stimuli.

Keywords Blindsight · Hemianopia · Redundancy gain ·

Subcortical visual pathways · Reaction time

Introduction

The term blindsight refers to unconscious visually guided behavior in response to stimuli presented to the blind hemi-field of patients with complete damage to the primary visual cortex (V1) with or without additional damage to extrastriate visual areas. This phenomenon was originally described in the mid-1970s (Poppel et al. 1973; Weisk-rantz et al. 1974) and subsequently was thoroughly and ingeniously pursued by Weiskrantz and various collabora-tors (see Weiskrantz 1990, 1996, 2004). The interest for this peculiar phenomenon concerns a challenging topic of cognitive neuroscience such as the neural correlate of per-ceptual awareness and also the function of visual pathways alternative to V1, not to mention the possibility that it might represent an initial step for regaining conscious vision (for recent reviews see Leopold 2012; Overgaard 2011; Perez and Chokron 2014; Silvanto 2014; Urbanski et al. 2014).

Abstract One important, yet relatively unexplored

ques-tion is whether blindsight, i.e., unconscious visually guided behavior in hemianopic patients, is endowed with basic perceptual properties such as detecting stimulus numerosity and overall configuration. Rather than a forced-choice pro-cedure in which patients are supposed to guess about stim-uli presented to the blind hemifield, we used a redundant signal effect paradigm, i.e., the speeding of simple reaction time (RT) when presenting multiple versus single similar stimuli. The presence of an effect of numerosity for the (unseen) stimuli presented to the blind field was indirectly assessed by measuring RT to bilateral versus unilateral stimuli presented to the intact hemifield. Chronic hemian-opic patients were tested with unilateral or bilateral black

A. Celeghin · S. Savazzi · M. Barabas · C. A. Marzi

Physiology and Psychology Section, Department of Neurological and Movement Sciences, University of Verona, Verona, Italy A. Celeghin (*)

Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience Laboratory and Center of Research on Psychology in Somatic Diseases (CoRPS), Tilburg University, PO Box 90153, 5000 LE Tilburg, The Netherlands

e-mail: A.Celeghin@uvt.nl; alessia.celeghin@unito.it A. Celeghin

Department of Psychology, University of Torino, Torino, Italy S. Savazzi · C. A. Marzi

National Institute of Neuroscience, Verona, Italy M. Barabas

Division of Neuropsychology, Department of Cognitive Neurology, Centre for Neurology, Hertie-Institute for Clinical Brain Research, Eberhard, Karls University, Tübingen, German M. Bendini

Department of Neuroradiology, Treviso Hospital, Treviso, Italy

(5)

Exp Brain Res

1 3

The presence of blindsight can be assessed either with direct methods, notably with a forced-choice response, or with indirect methods (see Danckert and Rossetti 2005 for review). In the former procedure, patients are asked to guess about the location or other attributes of stimuli presented to the blind hemifield and the presence of unconscious above chance performance is taken as evidence of blindsight. In the latter, patients are asked to respond to stimuli presented to the intact hemifield during (or following) stimulus pres-entation to the blind hemifield and blindsight is inferred by the influence of blind field stimulation in response to stimuli presented to the intact field. A major advantage of the latter method is that the patient is not forced to guess, and stimuli are visible on every trial in the good field.

One of the first examples of an indirect method for reveal-ing blindsight has been provided by an interfield summation task originally described by Marzi et al. (1986) by using a modified redundant signal effect (RSE) paradigm. Briefly, in this paradigm, single or double visual stimuli are tachisto-scopically presented in random sequence to one or both visual hemifields. Participants are required to manually press a key as quickly as possible following detection of either kind of stimuli, that is, unilateral (left or right) or bilateral stimuli pre-sented simultaneously across the vertical meridian, without having to make a choice (simple reaction time paradigm-RT). Marzi et al. (1986) found that there was a RSE, i.e., faster RT for double as compared to single stimuli, not only in control participants with an intact visual field but also in some hemi-anopic patients in whom bilateral stimuli were perceived as a single stimulus in the good field. The presence of this form of unconscious RSE has been confirmed several times (Corbetta et al. 1990; Leh et al. 2010; Marzi et al. 2009; Tamietto et al.

2010; Tomaiuolo et al. 1997), and there is functional mag-netic resonance imaging evidence for an important role of the superior colliculus (SC) in mediating this form of blindsight (Leh et al. 2010; Tamietto et al. 2010).

This kind of indirect method has proven to be a reliable indicator of blindsight but is quite conservative and usu-ally only a relatively small fraction of patients has shown clear indications of this phenomenon. Here we report the results of a study using a paradigm which represents a

modification of the original RSE wherein quadruple (rather than double) stimuli were displayed to each hemifield in alternative to single stimuli during either unilateral or bilat-eral simultaneous presentation. Perhaps more importantly, a novel addition to this paradigm is the use of two different configurations of quadruple stimuli, namely a structured and a randomized dot configuration. The former was con-stituted by four dots forming a diamond shape, while the latter was a randomized configuration changing on every trial. Thus, the two configurations differed in familiarity as well as in overall gestaltic structure and either of them might be important factors for revealing blindsight. Pre-vious evidence on the sensitivity of blindsight for overall stimulus configuration and other higher-order aspects is rather scant and relies mainly on hemianopic completion across the vertical meridian (e.g., Torjussen 1978; Weil et al. 2009) or on implicit semantic priming (e.g., Marcel

1998). Moreover, it is worth mentioning that a recent sin-gle-cell recording study in monkeys has demonstrated that SC neurons are sensitive to face and face-like configura-tions (Nguyen et al. 2014). All these studies encouraged us to try and test the possibility of finding gestalt-like as well as familiarity effects in blindsight.

Method

Participants

We tested six patients (mean age 45.83 years; SD 14.36) with hemianopia as a result of cortical or optic tract lesion, see details in Table 1 and Fig. 1. All patients were right-handed, and their acuity was normal or corrected-to-normal. They provided informed consent, and the study was approved by our Departmental Ethics Committee and was carried out along the principles laid down by the Helsinki Declaration. Stimuli and apparatus

The stimuli were black dots on a gray background of 11.42 cd/m2 luminance presented with an exposure duration Table 1 Demographic and clinical data of the patients

Sex Age Homonymous hemianopia

Etiology Lesions P1 M 23 Left Close head trauma Right optic tract P2 M 55 Left Ischemic stroke with hemorrhagic evolution Right parietal-occipital P3 M 36 Left Ischemic stroke Right temporal-occipital P4 M 60 Left Ischemic stroke Right mesial occipital

P5 F 44 Right Ischemic stroke with hemorrhagic evolution Medial part of left occipital lobe P6 M 57 Right Cerebral intraparenchymal hemorrhage Left parietal-occipital

(6)

Exp Brain Res

1 3

Fig. 1 Patients details. a Monocular visual fields showing a typical hemianopic loss with some spared areas, b structural MRI showing the

lesion, c functional MRI with full-field visual stimulation with checkerboards showing activation restricted to the contralesional visual cortex

(7)

Exp Brain Res

1 3

of 80 ms on a PC monitor. They could be either single or quadruple and could be presented either unilaterally or bilat-erally. A unilateral single stimulus was presented along the horizontal meridian at an eccentricity of 6.5° from a cen-tral fixation point either in the left or the right hemifield. In some patients, it has been necessary to present the stimuli a few degrees more laterally along the horizontal meridian to avoid encroaching upon partially spared portions of the vis-ual field. Bilateral single stimuli were presented in the two hemifields simultaneously at the same eccentricity as unilat-eral single stimuli. The experiment was divided in two ses-sions. In the first, the quadruple stimuli were four dots iden-tical to the single stimuli and were presented in a diamond configuration. In the second, we used the same four stimuli as in the first session but their position was randomized trial-by-trial so that they did not form a meaningful figure, see an example of the two configurations in Fig. 2. The ran-domized stimuli were presented always in the second ses-sion, but this did not yield any reliable practice effect, see ‘Results and discussion’. For both configurations, the center

was at 8.5° along the horizontal meridian and the innermost dot was at 6.5°.

Procedure

It is important to point out that unilateral stimuli presented to the blind field were not perceived by the patients who were requested to press the response key only when they were aware of the stimuli even if in a degraded and faint form. This did not occur in the above six patients with the exception of a few responses in patient SL. To further enquire about stimulus awareness, at the end of each ses-sion patients were asked if they had experienced or felt the occurrence of stimuli in the blind hemifield even when they did not respond. None of them reported having had such experience.

As mentioned above, the strategy followed to infer blindsight was to assess the effect of stimuli in the blind field on RT to stimuli perceived in the intact field.

Fig. 2 Examples of stimuli

and their spatial organization.

a Upper left unilateral single,

upper right bilateral single, b

middle left unilateral quadruple diamond, middle right bilateral quadruple diamond, c lower left unilateral quadruple random,

lower right bilateral quadruple random

(8)

Exp Brain Res

1 3

Unilateral stimuli presented to the intact hemifield served as control condition.

The initial event in a trial was a warning acoustic sig-nal (duration 150 ms; frequency 1000 Hz) followed after a randomized temporal window (300–700 ms) by a single or a quadruple stimulus presented to the right or left hemi-field or bilaterally. There were two sessions, one with dia-monds as stimulus configuration and the other with rand-omized stimuli. Each session included 80 unilateral single stimuli and 80 unilateral quadruple stimuli, half to the left and half to the right hemifield (in a blocked alternation), 40 bilateral single stimuli and 40 bilateral quadruple stimuli in addition to 40 catch trials where the warning stimulus was not followed by a visual stimulus. The sequence of unilat-eral, bilatunilat-eral, single or quadruple stimuli was randomized. Response was performed by pressing the spacing bar of a PC either with the left or the right hand according to a blocked ABBA sequence for each visual hemifield pres-entation. Participants were required to keep their fixation steady on a small black circle (diameter 0.3°) in the center of the visual field following onset of the warning signal and to refrain from moving the eyes until response had been completed. Fixation was remotely controlled by means of a closed TV system. RTs shorter than 140 ms (anticipations) and longer than 600 ms (retardations) were not included in the statistical analysis. Their overall percentage was <5 %.

Results and discussion

We carried out a repeated-measures ANOVA on RT aver-aged across sessions with ‘Configuration’ (Diamond vs. Random), ‘Numerosity in the Intact Field’ (1 vs. 4) and ‘Numerosity in the Blind Field’ (0, 1, 4) as factors. The lat-ter condition was represented by the following three levels: ‘0’, that is, unilateral stimuli (either single or quadruple) in the intact field and no stimuli in the blind field; ‘1’ bilateral

single stimuli; ‘4’ bilateral quadruple stimuli. Therefore, we could compare the effect of 0, 1 or 4 stimuli presented to the blind field on RT to stimuli presented to the intact field. Obviously, the level ‘0’ stimuli was not present in the ‘Numerosity in the Intact Field’ condition because patients would not perceive anything and would not press the response key in this condition.

Results showed a significant main effect of ‘Numer-osity in the Intact Field’ [F(1,5) = 37.384, p < .002] with quadruple stimuli (330.93 ms) faster than single stimuli (338.19 ms) and of ‘Numerosity in the Blind field’ [F(2,10) = 6.113, p < .018] with quadruple stimuli (332.40 ms) faster than single (335.05 ms) and 0 stim-uli (336.23 ms) that did not differ one from the other. The main effect of ‘Configuration’ was not significant [F(1,5) = .331, p = .589] with the Random (332.35 ms) not reliably faster than the Diamond Configuration (336.77 ms). Importantly, we found a significant effect of the ‘Configuration’ by ‘Numerosity in the Blind Field’ interaction [F(2,10) = 4.214, p < .05], while ‘Configura-tion’ by ‘Numerosity in the Intact Field’ was not significant [F(1,5) = 2.332, p = .187]. Finally, the three-ways inter-action ‘Configuration’ by ‘Numerosity in the Intact Field’ by ‘Numerosity in the Blind Field’ was not significant [F(2,10) = .0137, p = .986].

In order to analyze the ‘Configuration by Numerosity in the Blind Field’ interaction, two ANOVAs were carried out separately for each ‘Configuration’ (Diamond and Ran-dom) with ‘Numerosity in the Intact Field’ (1 and 4 stimuli) and ‘Numerosity in the Blind Field’ (0, 1 and 4 stimuli) as factors.

For the Diamond configuration, there was a significant effect of ‘Numerosity in the Intact Field’ [F(1,5) = 28.852,

p = .003], indicating that four stimuli were reacted faster than 1. Importantly, we also found a significant effect of ‘Numerosity in the Blind Field’ [F(2,10) = 7.477,

p = .010], indicating that RT (to stimuli in the intact

Fig. 3 RT to stimuli presented

to the intact field (IF) as a func-tion of stimulus numerosity (0, 1, 4) in the blind field (BF) for the diamond (a) and random configurations (b). On each panel: left single stimuli, right quadruple stimuli. Bars repre-sent standard errors (SE)

(9)

Exp Brain Res

1 3

field—the only ones to which patients could respond) decreased by increasing the number of stimuli in the blind field, see Fig. 3 left panel. A polynomial analysis showed that the decrease in RT was significantly linearly related (p = .008) to the increase in the stimuli in the blind field. Moreover, no interaction was found [F(2,10) = .060,

p = .942], indicating that the speeding up of RT induced by the number of stimuli presented in the blind field was unre-lated to the number of stimuli simultaneously presented in the intact field.

In contrast, for the Random configuration, the only significant effect was found for the ‘Numerosity in the Intact Field’ factor [F(1,5) = 8.651, p = .032], indicating that quadruple stimuli presented in the intact field were reacted faster than single stimuli. No effect was found for ‘Numerosity in the Blind Field’ [F(2,10) = .150, p = .863], see Fig. 3 right panel, or for the interaction ‘Numeros-ity in the Intact Field’ by ‘Numeros‘Numeros-ity in the Blind Field’ [F(2,10) = .245, p = .787].

The main thrust of these results is that increasing the number of stimuli in the blind field from 0 to one and to four, independently from the number of stimuli in the intact field, resulted in a progressive shortening of RT. This is clearly an implicit bilateral advantage in which unseen stimuli in the blind field are summated with normally per-ceived stimuli in the intact field. An intriguing finding sup-ported by the significant interaction ‘Configuration’ by ‘Numerosity in the Blind Field’ is that the bilateral advan-tage was reliable only for the diamond rather than for the random configuration of quadruple stimuli. This suggests the fascinating possibility that unconscious vision might have a perceptual organization enhancing the processing of structured over meaningless stimuli. An important point is that a practice effect explaining the failure to find blind-sight for the random stimuli session that was run always after the diamond stimuli session is an unlikely possibility given the lack of significance of the main effect of ‘Config-uration’ and of the three-ways interaction ‘Config‘Config-uration’ by ‘Numerosity in the Intact Field’ by ‘Numerosity in the Blind Field’.

As to the possible neural sites subserving the implicit effect described here, one should note that with the excep-tion of patient P5 who has a lesion mainly restricted to V1, all other patients have either large lesions extending to extrastriate areas and often to the optic radiation and sub-cortical centers. Therefore, it is likely that the implicit RSE found might have a subcortical site, as argued by previous work with roughly similar paradigms (Tomaiuolo et al.

1997; Leh et al. 2010; Tamietto et al. 2010). This would suggest the presence of higher-order perceptual effects even when vision in the absence of the geniculate-striate pathway is subserved by subcortical centers and its extras-triate cortical projections.

An important question concerns the contribution of the intact hemisphere either at cortical or subcortical level. This is supported by a recent study (Celeghin et al. 2015) in which we used in hemianopic patients a behavioral paradigm originally developed by Poffenberger in 1912 for measuring interhemispheric transfer time (see Marzi et al. 1991; Marzi 1999). We found that unlike responses to stimuli presented to the intact hemifield of hemianopics, those to stimuli presented to the blind hemifield (patients were instructed to respond also to unseen stimuli) showed a paradoxically slower uncrossed than crossed visuomotor response. That is, responding with the hand ipsilateral to the stimulated hemifield (an uncrossed condition that does not require an interhemisphere transfer) was slower than the crossed condition in which responses are performed with the hand contralateral to the stimulated hemifield, i.e., a condition which does require an interhemispheric transfer (see Marzi et al. 1991; Marzi 1999). This indicates that responses to the blind hemifield were mediated by the intact hemisphere via a double crossing of the corpus cal-losum that is necessary first to access the visual informa-tion from the intact visual cortex and then to trigger the motor response from the ipsilesional side, see Fig. 5 in Celeghin et al. 2015. Also, diffusion tensor imaging studies in blindsight patients provide converging evidence of post-lesion plasticity that involves subcortical visual structures such as the SC or the lateral geniculate nucleus with aber-rant fiber tracts reaching the intact hemisphere (e.g., Bridge et al. 2008; Leh et al. 2006; Tamietto et al. 2012). Finally, of particular interest is patient P1 of the present study who sustained a closed head trauma and a unilateral lesion of the optic tract completely depriving the ipsilesional hemi-sphere of visual input. In this case, blindsight could have been possible only through an aberrant (presumably post-lesional) misrouting of retinal fibers at the optic chiasm relying ipsilateral hemifield input to the intact hemisphere. Clearly, this input yielded blindsight but not conscious vision. The reason for that is an interesting issue to be fur-ther pursued.

Conclusions

The overall picture stemming from these results is that there was a clear cut speeding of RT with increasing numerosity of black dots. This effect occurred both in the intact hemifield and bilaterally despite the presence of a blind hemifield. This implicit RSE confirms earlier findings with bilateral single stimuli (Corbetta et al. 1990; Marzi et al. 1986, 2009; Tomaiuolo et al. 1997; Tamietto et al.

2010). The novel finding is that the present implicit RSE occurs only with quadruple stimuli with a structured rather than a random spatial configuration.

(10)

Exp Brain Res

1 3

Clearly, this result awaits confirmation by using vari-ous kinds of structured stimuli since in the present study the diamond configuration was the same in all trials while randomized stimuli varied from trial to trial. Thus, in prin-ciple, it could be argued that the observed implicit RSE was related to familiarity and/or to fixed versus variable stimuli rather than to the presence of a gestalt-like dot configura-tion. These possibilities need to be tested in further experi-ments and might provide further clues on the ‘cognitive’ structure of blindsight.

Acknowledgments The study was in part supported by ERC

Advanced Grant ‘Perceptual Awareness’ (C.A.M).

Conflict of interest The authors declare that they have no conflict

of interest.

References

Bridge H, Thomas O, Jbabdi S, Cowey A (2008) Changes in connec-tivity after visual cortical brain damage underlie altered visual function. Brain 13:1433–1444

Celeghin A, Barabas M, Mancini F, Bendini M, Pedrotti E, Prior M, Cantagallo A, Savazzi S, Marzi CA (2015) Speeded manual responses to unseen visual stimuli in hemianopic patients: what kind of blindsight? Conscious Cogn 32:6–14

Corbetta M, Marzi C, Tassinari G, Aglioti S (1990) Effective-ness of different task paradigms in revealing blindsight. Brain 113:603–616

Danckert J, Rossetti Y (2005) Blindsight in action: what can the dif-ferent sub-types of blindsight tell us about the control of visually guided actions? Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29:1035–1046

Leh SE, Johansen-Berg H, Ptito A (2006) Unconscious vision: new insights into the neuronal correlate of blindsight using diffusion tractography. Brain 129:1822–1832

Leh SE, Ptito A, Schönwiesner M, Chakravarty MM, Mullen KT (2010) Blindsight mediated by an S-cone-independent collicular pathway: an fMRI study in hemispherectomized subjects. J Cogn Neurosci 22:670–682. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21217

Leopold DA (2012) Primary visual cortex: awareness and blindsight. Annu Rev Neurosci 35:91–109

Marcel AJ (1998) Blindsight and shape perception: deficit of visual consciousness or of visual function? Brain 121:1565–1588 Marzi CA (1999) The Poffenberger paradigm: a first, simple,

behavio-ral tool to study interhemispheric transmission in humans. Brain Res Bull 50:421–422

Marzi CA, Tassinari G, Aglioti S, Lutzemberger L (1986) Spatial summation across the vertical meridian in hemianopics: a test of blindsight. Neuropsychologia 24:749–758

Marzi CA, Bisiacchi P, Nicoletti R (1991) Is interhemispheric transfer of visuomotor information asymmetric? Evidence from a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 29:1163–1177

Marzi CA, Mancini F, Metitieri T, Savazzi S (2009) Blindsight fol-lowing visual cortex deafferentation disappears with purple and red stimuli: a case study. Neuropsychologia 47:1382–1385. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.01.023

Nguyen MN, Mtsumoto J, Hori E, Maior RS, Tomaz C, Tran AH, Ono T, Nishijo H (2014) Neuronal responses to face-like and facial stimuli in the monkey superior colliculus. Front Behav Neurosci 8:85. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00085 (eCollection 2014)

Overgaard M (2011) Visual experience and blindsight: a meth-odological review. Exp Brain Res 209:473–479. doi:10.1007/ s00221-011-2578-2

Perez C, Chokron S (2014) Rehabilitation of homonymous hemia-nopia: insight into blindsight. Front Integr Neurosci 8:82. doi:10.3389/fnint.2014.00082 (eCollection 2014)

Poppel E, Held R, Frost D (1973) Residual visual function after brain wounds involving the central visual pathways in man. Nature 243:295–296

Silvanto J (2014) Why is “blindsight” blind? A new perspective on primary visual cortex, recurrent activity and visual awareness. Conscious Cogn 32:15–32. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2014.08.001

Tamietto M, Cauda F, Corazzini LL, Savazzi S, Marzi CA, Goebel R, Weiskrantz L, de Gelder B (2010) Collicular vision guides non-conscious behavior. J Cogn Neurosci 22:888–902. doi:10.1162/ jocn.2009.21225

Tamietto M, Pullens P, de Gelder B, Weiskrantz L, Goebel R (2012) Subcortical connections to human amygdala and changes follow-ing destruction of the visual cortex. Curr Biol 22:1445–1449 Tomaiuolo F, Ptito M, Marzi CA, Paus T, Ptito A (1997) Blindsight

in hemispherectomized patients as revealed by spatial summation across the vertical meridian. Brain 120:795–803

Torjussen T (1978) Visual processing in cortically blind hemifields. Neuropsychologia 16:15–21

Urbanski M, Coubard OA, Bourlon C (2014) Visualizing the blind brain: brain imaging of visual field defects from early recov-ery to rehabilitation techniques. Front Integr Neurosci 8:74. doi:10.3389/fnint.2014.00074 (eCollection 2014)

Weil RS, Plant GT, James-Galton M, Rees G (2009) Neural correlates of hemianopic completion across the vertical meridian. Neuropsy-chologia 47:457–464. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.09.020

Weiskrantz L (1990) The Ferrier lecture, 1989. Outlooks for blind-sight: explicit methodologies for implicit processes. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 239:247–278

Weiskrantz L (1996) Blindsight revisited. Curr Opin Neurobiol 6:215–220

Weiskrantz L (2004) Roots of blindsight. Prog Brain Res 144:229–241 Weiskrantz L, Warrington EK, Sanders MD, Marshall J (1974) Visual

capacity in the hemianopic field following a restricted occipital ablation. Brain 97:709–728

Riferimenti

Documenti correlati

We supposed that the deep representation of one’s own body, and not merely its vision, is determinant to anticipate incoming sensory stimulation properly , thus we

Pond Use by Captive African Penguins (Spheniscus demersus) in an Immersive Exhibit Adjacent to Human Bathers..

Soon after, it was also recognised (Menten 1991b) that different pumping mech- anisms produce inversion in Class I methanol masers (CIMMs), first detected in a series of lines near 25

site FLUXNET ID, optical system (Manufacturer/Model), spectral range/channels (nm) and their resolution (FWHM, nm), the method used for acquisition (Single/Dual beam), the geometry

Two gas±steam combined cycles using thermodynamic analysis are presented: a combined cycle with three pressure levels with reheat heat recovery boiler is used with two di€erent

[r]

Ciononostante l’iniziativa europea riguardante la cooperazione energetica, e i vari programmi di parternariato che negli anni si sono succeduti tra l’UE e l’Azerbaigian

Il Codice Rocco del 1930 ha cercato di conciliare i principi della Scuola Classica e della Scuola Positiva, arrivando ad un sistema penale che non è riuscito